Acknowledgement
이 논문은 2017년 대한민국 교육부와 한국연구재단의 지원을 받아 수행된 연구임(NRF-2017S1A5A2A01024313).
References
- Andersen, S. M., Spielman, L. A., & Bargh, J. A. (1992). Future-event schemas and certainty about the future: Automaticity in depressives' future-event predictions. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 63(5), 711-723. DOI: 10.1037/0022-3514.63.5.711
- Anderson, B. A. (2018). Controlled information processing, automaticity, and the burden of proof. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 25(5), 1814-1823. DOI: 10.3758/s13423-017-1412-7
- Ansorge, U., & Wuhr, P. (2004). A response-discrimination account of the Simon effect. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 30(2), 365-377. DOI: 10.1037/0096- 1523.30.2.365
- Aquino, A., Paolini, D., Pagliaro, S., Migliorati, D., Wolff, A., Alparone, F. R., & Costantini, M. (2015). Group membership and social status modulate joint actions. Experimental Brain Research, 233(8), 2461-2466. DOI: 10.1007/s00221-015-4316-7
- Baddeley, A. D., & Hitch, G. (1974). Working memory. In G. H. Bower (Ed.), Psychology of Learning and Motivation (Vol. 8, pp. 47-89). Academic Press. DOI: 10.1016/S0079-7421(08)60452-1
- Bargh, J. A. (1994). The four horsemen of automaticity: Intention, awareness, efficiency, and control as separate issues. In R. S. Wyer & T. K. Srull (Eds.), Handbook of Social Cognition: Vol. 1, Basic Processes (2nd edn) (pp. 1-40). Erlbaum. DOI: 10.4324/9781315807102
- Belletier, C., Davranche, K., Tellier, I. S., Dumas, F., Vidal, F., Hasbroucq, T., & Huguet, P. (2015). Choking under monitoring pressure: Being watched by the experimenter reduces executive attention. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 22(5), 1410-1416. DOI: 10.3758/s13423-015-0804-9
- Benjamini, Y., & Hochberg, Y. (1995). Controlling the false discovery rate: A practical and powerful approach to multiple testing. Journal of the Royal Statistical Society: Series B (Methodological), 57(1), 289-300. DOI: 10.1111/j.2517-6161.1995.tb02031.x
- Borgmann, K. W. U., Risko, E. E., Stolz, J. A., & Besner, D. (2007). Simon says: Reliability and the role of working memory and attentional control in the Simon task. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 14(2), 313-319. DOI: 10.3758/bf03194070
- Brainard, D. H. (1997). The Psychophysics toolbox. Spatial Vision, 10(4), 433-436. DOI: 10.1163/156856897x00357
- Brass, M., Bekkering, H., & Prinz, W. (2001). Movement observation affects movement execution in a simple response task. Acta Psychologica, 106(1-2), 3-22. DOI: 10.1016/s0001-6918(00)00024-x
- Chartrand, T. L., & Bargh, J. A. (1999). The chameleon effect: The perception-behavior link and social interaction. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 76(6), 893-910. DOI: 10.1037/0022-3514.76.6.893
- Colzato, L. S., De Bruijn, E., & Hommel, B. (2012). Up to "me" or up to "us"? The impact of self-construal priming on cognitive self-other integration. Frontiers in Psychology, 3. DOI: 10.3389/fpsyg.2012.00341
- Costantini, M., & Ferri, F. (2013). Action co-representation and social exclusion. Experimental Brain Research, 227(1), 85-92. DOI: 10.1007/s00221-013-3487-3
- De Jong, R., Liang, C. C., & Lauber, E. (1994). Conditional and unconditional automaticity: A dual-process model of effects of spatial stimulus-response correspondence. Journal of Experimental Psychology. Human Perception and Performance, 20(4), 731-750. DOI: 10.1037//0096-1523.20.4.731
- Dolk, T., Hommel, B., Colzato, L. S., Schutz-Bosbach, S., Prinz, W., & Liepelt, R. (2014). The joint Simon effect: A review and theoretical integration. Frontiers in Psychology, 5, 974. DOI: 10.3389/fpsyg.2014.00974
- Dolk, T., Hommel, B., Prinz, W., & Liepelt, R. (2013). The (not so) social Simon effect: A referential coding account. Journal of Experimental Psychology. Human Perception and Performance, 39(5), 1248-1260. DOI: 10.1037/a0031031
- Efron, B., & Tibshirani, R. J. (1994). An Introduction to the bootstrap. CRC Press. DOI: 10.1201/9780429246593
- Frossard, J., & Renaud, O. (2021). Permutation tests for regression, ANOVA, and comparison of signals: The permuco package. Journal of Statistical Software, 99, 1-32. DOI: 10.18637/jss.v099.i15
- Guagnano, D., Rusconi, E., & Umilta, C. A. (2010). Sharing a task or sharing space? On the effect of the confederate in action coding in a detection task. Cognition, 114(3), 348-355. DOI: 10.1016/j.cognition.2009.10.008
- Hermans, D., Crombez, G., & Eelen, P. (2000). Automatic attitude activation and efficiency: The fourth horseman of automaticity. Psychologica Belgica, 40(1), 3-22. DOI: 10.5334/pb.954
- Hommel, B. (1993). Inverting the Simon effect by intention: Determinants of direction and extent of effects of irrelevant spatial information. Psychological Research, 55(4), 270-279. DOI: 10.1007/bf00419687
- Hommel, B., Colzato, L. S., & van den Wildenberg, W. P. M. (2009). How social are task representations? Psychological Science, 20(7), 794-798. DOI: 10.1111/j.1467-9280.2009.02367.x
- Kahneman, D., & Treisman, A. (1984). Changing views of attention and automaticity. In R. Parasuraman & D. R. Davis (Eds.), Varieties of attention (pp. 29-61). Academic Press.
- Kane, M. J., & Engle, R. W. (2003). Working-memory capacity and the control of attention: The contributions of goal neglect, response competition, and task set to Stroop interference. Journal of Experimental Psychology. General, 132(1), 47-70. DOI: 10.1037/0096-3445.132.1.47
- Karlinsky, A., Lohse, K., & Lam, M. (2017). A meta-analysis of the joint Simon effect. Annual Meeting of the Cognitive Science Society, 1, 2377-2382. https://cogsci.mindmodeling.org/2017/papers/0452/paper0452.pdf
- Kim, G., Cho, S., & Hyun, J.-S. (2015). A study of individual differences across numerosity sensitivity, visual working memory and visual attention. Science of Emotion and Sensibility, 18(2), 3-18. DOI: 10.14695/KJSOS.2015.18.2.3
- Knoblich, G., & Sebanz, N. (2006). The social nature of perception and action. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 15(3), 99-104. DOI: 10.1111/ j.0963-7214.2006.00415.x
- Kornblum, S., Hasbroucq, T., & Osman, A. (1990). Dimensional overlap: Cognitive basis for stimulus-response compatibility-a model and taxonomy. Psychological Review, 97(2), 253-270. DOI: 10.1037/0033-295x.97.2.253
- Lakens, D., & Caldwell, A. R. (2021). Simulation-based power analysis for factorial analysis of variance designs. Advances in Methods and Practices in Psychological Science, 4(1), 2515245920951503. DOI: 10.1177/2515245920951503
- Lee, H., & Kim, J. (2018). Effects of emotional information on visual perception and working memory in biological motion. Science of Emotion and Sensibility, 21(3), 151-164. DOI: 10.14695/KJSOS.2018.21.3.151
- Logan, G. D., & Cowan, W. B. (1984). On the ability to inhibit thought and action: A theory of an act of control. Psychological Review, 91(3), 295. DOI:10.1037/ 0033-295X.91.3.295
- Lu, C. H., & Proctor, R. W. (1995). The influence of irrelevant location information on performance: A review of the Simon and spatial Stroop effects. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 2(2), 174-207. DOI: 10.3758/BF03210959
- Lupfer, M. B., Clark, L. F., & Hutcherson, H. W. (1990). Impact of context on spontaneous trait and situational attributions. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 58(2), 239-249. DOI: 10.1037/0022-3514.58.2.239
- McClung, J. S., Jentzsch, I., & Reicher, S. D. (2013). Group membership affects spontaneous mental representation: failure to represent the out-group in a joint action task. PloS One, 8(11), e79178. DOI: 10.1371/journal.pone.0079178
- Michel, R., Bolte, J., & Liepelt, R. (2018). When a social experimenter overwrites effects of salient objects in an individual go/no-go Simon task - An ERP study. Frontiers in Psychology, 9. DOI: 10.3389/fpsyg.2018.00674
- Muller, B. C. N., Kuhn, S., van Baaren, R. B., Dotsch, R., Brass, M., & Dijksterhuis, A. (2011). Perspective taking eliminates differences in co-representation of out-group members' actions. Experimental Brain Research, 211(3-4), 423-428. DOI: 10.1007/s00221-011-2654-7
- Pelli, D. G. (1997). The VideoToolbox software for visual psychophysics: Transforming numbers into movies. Spatial Vision, 10(4), 437-442. DOI: 10.1163/156856897x00366
- R Core Team. (2020). R: A language and environment for statistical computing. Vienna, Austria. Retrieved from https://www.R-project.org/
- Ryu, J.-H. (2009). The impact of cognitive load factors and arousal levels of galvanic skin response on task performance in computer based learning. Science of Emotion and Sensibility, 12(3), 279-288. https://www.koreascience.or.kr/article/JAKO200935535844977.page
- Ryu, J.-H. (2010). Impact of picture and reading mode on cognitive load and galvanic skin response. Science of Emotion and Sensibility, 13(1), 21-32. https://www.koreascience.or.kr/article/JAKO201023557658745.page 1023557658745.page
- Sebanz, N., Bekkering, H., & Knoblich, G. (2006). Joint action: Bodies and minds moving together. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 10(2), 70-76. DOI: 10.1016/j.tics.2005.12.009
- Sebanz, N., Knoblich, G., & Prinz, W. (2003). Representing others' actions: Just like one's own? Cognition, 88(3), B11-B21. DOI: 10.1016/s0010-0277(03)00043-x
- Siegel, S., & Castellan, J. N. (1988). Nonparametric statistics for the behavioral sciences. McGraw-Hill. DOI: 10.4135/9781412961288.n273
- Simon, J. R., & Rudell, A. P. (1967). Auditory SR compatibility: The effect of an irrelevant cue on information processing. The Journal of Applied Psychology, 51(3), 300. DOI: 10.1037/h0020586
- Stroop, J. R. (1935). Studies of interference in serial verbal reactions. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 18(6), 643. DOI: 10.1037/h0054651
- Welsh, T. N., Kiernan, D., Neyedli, H. F., Ray, M., Pratt, J., Potruff, A., & Weeks, D. J. (2013). Joint Simon effects in extrapersonal space. Journal of Motor Behavior, 45(1), 1-5. DOI: 10.1080/00222895.2012.746635
- Wigboldus, D. H. J., Sherman, J. W., Franzese, H. L., & van Knippenberg, A. (2004). Capacity and comprehension: Spontaneous stereotyping under cognitive load. Social Cognition, 22(3), 292-309. DOI: 10.1521/soco.22.3.292.35967
- Wuhr, P., & Ansorge, U. (2007). A Simon ef f ect in memory retrieval: Evidence for the response-discrimination account. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 14(5), 984-988. DOI: 10.3758/bf03194132
- Wuhr, P., & Biebl, R. (2011). The role of working memory in spatial S-R correspondence effects. Journal of Experimental Psychology. Human Perception and Performance, 37(2), 442-454. DOI: 10.1037/a0020563
- Yantis, S., & Jonides, J. (1990). Abrupt visual onsets and selective attention: voluntary versus automatic allocation. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 16(1), 121-134. DOI: 10.1037/0096-1523.16.1.121
- Zhao, X., Chen, A., & West, R. (2010). The influence of working memory load on the Simon effect. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 17(5), 687-692. DOI: 10.3758/PBR.17.5.687