DOI QR코드

DOI QR Code

또래 놀이에 나타난 유아의 맥락 및 탈맥락적 언어사용

Use of Contextualized and Decontextualized Talk in Preschoolers' Peer Play

  • 투고 : 2014.07.08
  • 심사 : 2014.09.15
  • 발행 : 2014.10.31

초록

The purpose of this study is to investigate low- and middle-income preschoolers' use of contextualized and decontextualized talk. The contextualized and decontextualized talk of 70 five-year-old children were analyzed by children's gender and their socioeconomic status. The participants, as dyads, participated in a peer play session for 10 minutes. The play sessions were videotaped and the videotaped data were transcribed. Each transcript was analyzed according to the modified version of Curenton et al. (2008)'s coding schemes. The results showed that children from middle-income families engaged more in both contextualized and decontextualized talk than children from low-income families. Also, girls from middle-income families engaged in more contextualized-decontextualized talk(psychological states) than girls from low-income families.

키워드

참고문헌

  1. Curenton, S. M., & Justice, L. M. (2004). African American and Caucasian preschoolers' use of decontextualized language: Literate language features in oral narratives. Language, Speech, and Hearing Services in Schools, 35, 240-253. https://doi.org/10.1044/0161-1461(2004/023)
  2. Curenton, S. M., & Lucas, T. D. (2007). Assessing narrative development. In K. Phence (Ed.), Assessment in emergent and early literacy (pp. 377-432). San Diego, CA: Plural.
  3. Curenton, S. M., Craig, M. J., & Flanigan, N. (2008). Use of decontextualized talk across story contexts: How oral storytelling and emergent reading can scaffold children's development. Early Education and Development, 19(1), 161-187. https://doi.org/10.1080/10409280701839296
  4. Davidson, R. G., & Snow, C. E. (1995). The linguistic environment of early readers. Journal of Research in Childhood Education, 10, 5-21. https://doi.org/10.1080/02568549509594683
  5. Dickinson, D. K., & Snow, C. E. (1987). Interrelationships among prereading and oral language skills in kindergartners from two social classes. Early Childhood Research Quarterly, 1, 1-26.
  6. Farran, D. C., & Haskins, R. (1980). Reciprocal influence in the social interactions of mothers and three-year-old children from different socioeconomic backgrounds. Child Development, 51, 780-791. https://doi.org/10.2307/1129465
  7. Feagans, L., & Farran, D. C. (1981). How demonstrated comprehension can get muddled in production. Developmental Psychology, 17, 718-727. https://doi.org/10.1037/0012-1649.17.6.718
  8. Greenhalgh, K. S., & Strong, C. J. (2001). Literate Language feature in spoken narratives of children with typical language and children with language impairments. Language, Speech, and Hearing Services in Schools, 32, 114-125. https://doi.org/10.1044/0161-1461(2001/010)
  9. Hart, B., & Risley, T. (1992). American parenting of language-learning children: Persisting differences in family-child interactions observed in natural home environments. Developmental Psychology, 28, 1096-1105. https://doi.org/10.1037/0012-1649.28.6.1096
  10. Heath, S. B. (1983). Ways with words: Language, life, and work in communities and classrooms. Cambridge, England: Cambridge University Press.
  11. Heath, S. B. (1994). What no bedtime story means: Narrative skills at home and school. In B. B. Schieffelin & E. Ochs (Eds.), Language socialization across cultures (pp.97-124). Cambridge, England: Cambridge University Press.
  12. Lee, Y., Lee, J., & Shin, E. (2002). Young children's pretense themes, self-other relations, and decontextualization of pretend play in block, manipulative, and dramatic play areas. Research in Education, 10, 29-51.
  13. Morgan, L., & Goldstein, H. (2004). Teaching mothers of low socioeconomic status to use decontextualized language during storybook reading. Journal of Early Intervention, 26, 235-252. https://doi.org/10.1177/105381510402600401
  14. Pellegrini, A. D. (1985). The relations between symbolic play and literate behavior: A review and critique of the empirical literature. Review of Educational Research, 55, 107-121. https://doi.org/10.3102/00346543055001107
  15. Reese, E. (1995). Predicting children's literacy from mother-child conversations. Cognitive Development, 10, 381-405. https://doi.org/10.1016/0885-2014(95)90003-9
  16. Rosenquest, B. B. (2002). Infants and toddlers literacy-based planning and pedagogy that supports toddler language development. Early Childhood Education Journal, 29, 241-249. https://doi.org/10.1023/A:1015133724460
  17. Rowe, M. L. (2013). Decontextualized language input and preschoolers' vocabulary development. Seminars in Speech and Language, 34(4), 260-266. https://doi.org/10.1055/s-0033-1353444
  18. Teale, W. H., & Sulzby, E. (1986). Emergent literacy as a perspective for examining how young children become writers and readers. In W. H. Teale & E. Sulzby (Eds.), Emergent literacy: Writing and reading (pp. 7-27). Norword, NJ: Ablex.
  19. Wanska, S. K., Bedrosian, J. L., & Pohlman, J. C. (1986). Effects of play materials on the topic performance of preschool children. Language, Speech and Hearing Services in Schools, 17, 152-159. https://doi.org/10.1044/0161-1461.1703.152
  20. Westby, C. E. (1991). Learning to talk, talking to learn: Oral literate language differences. In C. S. Simon (Ed.), Communication skills and classroom success (pp. 334-357). Eau Claire, WI: Thinking Publications.