DOI QR코드

DOI QR Code

Revisiting the Effect of Syllable Transposition in Korean Word Recognition: Disentangling Orthographic and Morphological Influences

한글 단어 재인에서 음절 전위 효과의 재검토: 표기 처리와 형태소 처리의 영향 분석

  • Sungbong, Bae (Department of Psychology, Yeungnam University) ;
  • Chang H. Lee (Department of Psychology, Sogang University)
  • 배성봉 (영남대학교 심리학과) ;
  • 이창환 (서강대학교 심리학과)
  • Received : 2024.06.30
  • Accepted : 2024.07.07
  • Published : 2024.09.30

Abstract

The letter transposition effect is crucial for understanding whether letter position coding within words is fixed. Despite the recognized importance of syllables in Korean word recognition, studies on syllable transposition effects have been inconsistent, indicating a lack of clarity on its mechanisms. Our study aims to address this by analyzing the syllable transposition effect, with a particular focus on distinguishing the influences of orthographic from morphological processing. This focus is due to Korean syllables serving simultaneously as units of orthography and elements of morphology. Through a masked priming lexical decision task with bisyllabic words, we conducted two experiments. Experiment 1 examined the effect across various word types to assess the impact of word origin, while Experiment 2 directly compared the influences of morphological and semantic processing. Results from both experiments showed a significant syllable transposition effect across all word types, pointing to orthographic processing as the key factor in the effect, rather than morphological or semantic factors. This underscores the flexibility of syllable position coding in the early stages of word processing and emphasizes orthographic processing as the primary influence on the syllable transposition effect.

문자 전위 효과는 단어 내 문자의 위치 부호화가 고정적인지 아니면 유동적인지를 규명하는 데 중요한 역할을 한다. 한국어 단어 재인에서 음절은 핵심적인 단위이지만, 음절 전위 효과에 대한 기존 연구들은 일관된 결과를 보이지 않는다. 이는 한글 단어 재인에서 음절 전위 효과의 메커니즘이 아직 충분히 이해되지 않았음을 나타낸다. 본 연구는 한글 표기 음절이 표기 단위이면서 동시에 형태소 단위로 기능한다는 점에 주목하여, 표기 처리와 형태소 처리의 영향을 분리하여 음절 전위 효과를 재검토하였다. 이를 위해 한글 표기 2음절 단어를 사용한 차폐 점화 어휘 판단 과제를 이용한 두 개의 실험을 진행했다. 실험 1에서는 고유어, 한자어, 외래어를 대상으로 음절 전위 효과를 검토해 어종의 영향을 측정했고, 실험 2에서는 단일어와 합성어를 활용해 형태소/의미적 처리가 음절 전위 효과에 미치는 영향을 직접적으로 비교했다. 실험 1과 2의 결과는 모두 2음절 단어에서 유의미한 음절 전위 효과를 분명하게 보여주었으며, 단어의 어종이나 합성성 여부에 관계없이 이 효과는 일관되게 관찰되었다. 이는 형태소/의미적 요인보다는 표기 처리가 한국어 단어 재인에서 음절 전위 효과를 주도한다는 것을 시사한다. 결과적으로, 본 연구는 한글 단어 재인의 초기 단계에서 음절 위치의 부호화가 유동적임을 보여주는 동시에, 음절 전위 효과가 의미 처리가 아니라 지각적인 표기 기반 처리에 의해 발생한다는 것을 의미한다.

Keywords

Acknowledgement

이 논문은 2021년 한국연구재단의 인문사회분야 중견연구자 사업의 지원을 받아 수행된 연구임 (NRF-2021S1A5A2A01060860).

References

  1. 김제홍, 이창환, 남기춘 (2018). 한국어 명사 어절 재인에서 나타나는 음절교환 효과. 한국심리학회지: 인지 및 생물, 30(3), 261-268. https://doi.org/10.22172/COGBIO.2018.30.3.004
  2. 박권생 (1995). 한글 단어 재인을 위한 시각 처리 과정. 한국심리학회지: 인지 및 생물, 7(2), 61-78.
  3. 이광오 (2009). 한국어 고유어에서의 형태소 처리. 한국심리학회지: 인지 및 생물, 21(3), 233-247. https://doi.org/10.22172/COGBIO.2009.21.3.005
  4. 이영애 (1990). 한글 낱자의 정보처리에 있어서 시각 변형의 효과. 인지과학 2(2), 221-259.
  5. 이창환 (2022). 한글 단어재인에 있어 음절교환 점화의 억제적 효과: 이웃단어의 조절 가능성 탐색. 언어과학, 29, 53-72.
  6. 배성봉, 이창환, 이윤형 (2021). 한국어 비단어의 어휘 판단에서 글자 전위 효과. 한국심리학회지: 인지 및 생물, 33(3), 163-176. https://doi.org/10.22172/COGBIO.2021.33.3.004
  7. 최원일, 김은서, 강진원 (2023). 한국어 문장 읽기 시의 음절 교환 효과: 안구 운동 추적 연구. 한국심리학회지: 인지 및 생물, 35(3), 205-212. https://doi.org/10.22172/COGBIO.2023.35.3.007
  8. Brysbaert, M., & Stevens, M. (2018). Power analysis and effect size in mixed effects models: A tutorial. Journal of Cognition, 1, 1-20.
  9. Burkner, P. C. (2017). brms: An R package for Bayesian multilevel models using Stan. Journal of statistical software, 80, 1-28.
  10. Cao, H. W., Chen, C., & Yan, H. M. (2021). Morpheme transposition of two-character Chinese words in vertical visual fields. Journal of Psycholinguistic Research, 1-16.
  11. Christianson, K., Johnson, R. L., & Rayner, K. (2005). Letter transpositions within and across morphemes. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 31(6), 1327.
  12. Coltheart, M., Rastle, K., Perry, C., Langdon, R., & Ziegler, J. (2001). DRC: a dual route cascaded model of visual word recognition and reading aloud. Psychological review, 108(1), 204-256.
  13. Davis, C. J. (2012). Developing a universal model of reading necessitates cracking the orthographic code. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, (5), 283.
  14. Davis, C., Kim, J., & Forster, K. I. (2008). Being forward not backward: Lexical limits to masked priming. Cognition, 107(2), 673-684.
  15. Davis, C. J. (2010). The spatial coding model of visual word identification. Psychological Review, 117(3), 713-758.
  16. Dunabeitia, J. A., Perea, M., & Carreiras, M. (2007). Do transposed-letter similarity effects occur at a morpheme level? Evidence for morpho-orthographic decomposition. Cognition, 105, 691-703. doi:10.1016/j.cognition.2006.12.001
  17. Dunabeitia, J. A., Perea, M., & Carreiras, M. (2009). There is no clam with coats in the calm coast: Delimiting the transposed-letter priming effect. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 62(10), 1930-1947.
  18. Dunabeitia, J. A., Perea, M. & Carreiras, M. (2014). Revisiting letter transpositions within and across morphemic boundaries. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 21, 1557-1575.
  19. Forster, K. I., & Forster, J. C. (2003). DMDX: A Windows display program with millisecond accuracy. Behavior research methods, instruments, & computers, 35, 116-124.
  20. Frost, R. (2012). A universal approach to modeling visual word recognition and reading: Not only possible, but also inevitable. The Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 35(5), 310-329.
  21. Gomez, P., Ratcliff, R., & Perea, M. (2008). The overlap model: A model of letter position coding. Psychological Review, 115(3), 577-600.
  22. Grainger, J., & Whitney, C. (2004). Does the huamn mnid raed wrods as a wlohe? Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 8(2), 58-59.
  23. Gu, J., & Li, X. (2015). The effects of character transposition within and across words in Chinese reading. Attention, Perception, & Psychophysics, 77(1), 272-281.
  24. Gu, J., Li, X., & Liversedge, S. P. (2015). Character order processing in Chinese reading. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 41(1), 127-137.
  25. Guerrera, C., & Forster, K. (2008). Masked form priming with extreme transposition. Language and Cognitive Processes, 23(1), 117-142.
  26. Kinoshita, S., & Norris, D. (2009). Transposed-letter priming of prelexical orthographic representations. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 35(1), 1-18.
  27. Korean Word Database (2001). 21th Century Sejong Project Corpus. The National Institute of the Korean Language Seoul: Korea.
  28. Lee, C. H., Kwon, Y., Kim, K., & Rastle, K. (2015). Syllable transposition effects in Korean word recognition. Journal of Psycholinguistic Research, 44(3), 309-315.
  29. Lee, C. H., Lally, C., & Rastle, K. (2021). Masked transposition priming effects are observed in Korean in the same-different task. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 74, 1-12.
  30. Lee, C. H., & Taft, M. (2009). Are onsets and codas important in processing letter position? A comparison of TL effects in English and Korean. Journal of Memory and Language, 60(4), 530-542.
  31. Lee, C. H., & Taft, M. (2011). Subsyllabic structure reflected in letter confusability effects in Korean word recognition. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 18(1), 129-134.
  32. Lin, Y. C., & Lin, P. Y. (2016). Mouse tracking traces the "Camrbidge Unievrsity" effects in monolingual and bilingual minds. Acta psychologica, 167, 52-62.
  33. Lupker, S. J., Perea, M., & Davis, C. J. (2008). Transposed-letter effects: Consonants, vowels and letter frequency. Language and Cognitive Processes, 23(1), 93-116.
  34. Lukatela, G., & Turvey, M. T. (1998). Reading in two alphabets. American Psychologist, 53(9), 1057-1072.
  35. McClelland, J. L., & Rumelhart, D. E. (1981). An interactive activation model of context effects in letter perception: I. An account of basic findings. Psychological Review, 88(5), 375-407.
  36. Norris, D., & Kinoshita, S. (2012). Reading through a noisy channel: Why there's nothing special about the perception of orthography. Psychological Review, 119(3), 517-545.
  37. Perea, M., & Lupker, S. J. (2003). Does jugde activate COURT? Transposed-letter similarity effects in masked associative priming. Memory & Cognition, 31(6), 829-841.
  38. Perea, M., & Lupker, S. J. (2004). Can CANISO activate CASINO? Transposed-letter similarity effects with nonadjacent letter positions. Journal of Memory and Language, 51(2), 231-246.
  39. Perea, M., & Perez, E. (2009). Beyond alphabetic orthographies: The role of form and phonology in transposition effects in Katakana. Language and Cognitive Processes, 24(1), 67-88.
  40. R Core Team (2021). R: A language and environment for statistical computing. R Foundation for Statistical Computing.
  41. Rastle, K., Lally, C., & Lee, C. H. (2019). No flexibility in letter position coding in Korean. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 45(4), 458-473.
  42. Sanchez-Gutierrez, C., & Rastle, K. (2013). Letter transpositions within and across morphemic boundaries: Is there a crosslanguage difference? Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 20, 988-996.
  43. Schoonbaert, S., & Grainger, J. (2004). Letter position coding in printed word perception: Effects of repeated and transposed letters. Language and Cognitive Processes, 19(3), 333-367.
  44. Yang, H., Chen, J., Spinelli, G., & Lupker, S. J. (2019). The impact of text orientation on form priming effects in four-character Chinese words. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 45(8), 1511-1526.