References
- Ministry of Education (2019a). Mathematics 5-1. Chunjae Education.
- Ministry of Education (2019b). Mathematics 5-2. Chunjae Education.
- Ministry of Education (2019c). Mathematics 6-1. Chunjae Education.
- Ministry of Education (2019d). Mathematics 6-2. Chunjae Education.
- Ministry of Education (2019e). Mathematics workbook 5-1. Chunjae Education.
- Ministry of Education (2019f). Mathematics workbook 5-2. Chunjae Education.
- Ministry of Education (2019g). Mathematics workbook 6-1. Chunjae Education.
- Ministry of Education (2019h). Mathematics workbook 6-2. Chunjae Education.
- Ministry of Education (2022.10.11.). The 1st comprehensive plan for basic academic skills guarantee. Ministry of Education.
- Lan, A. D, & Lee. H. S. (2020). Why they chose What they chose: Exploring effects of test performance and metacognitive judgments on learners' selection of interleaving schedule. Korean Journal of Cognitive and Biological Psychology, 32(2), 169-188. https://doi.org/10.22172/COGBIO.2020.32.2.004
- Ryu, J. M., & Kang, E. C. (2021). Interaction effect of retrieval practice methods and math achievement levels in math learning. The Journal of Thinking Development, 17(2), 23-42.
- Pang, J. S., Kim, L. N., & Kim, S. H. (2022). A comparative analysis of introducing addition and subtraction in the Korean, Singaporean, American, and Japanese elementary textbooks. Communication of Mathematics Education, 36(2), 229-252.
- Shin, H. Y., & Park, S. Y. (2023). Possibilities and limitations of basic academic skills support policy : Focusing on the 1st comprehensive plan for basic academic skills guarantee. The Journal of Politics of Education, 30(1), 1-30. https://doi.org/10.52183/KSPE.2023.30.1.1
- Bjork, R. A., & Bjork, E. L. (2020). Desirable difficulties in theory and practice. Journal of Applied Research in Memory and Cognition, 9(4), 475-479. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jarmac.2020.09.003
- Brown, P. C., Roediger, H. L. III, & McDaniel, M. A. (2014). Make it stick: The science of successful learning. Belknap Press of Harvard University Press.
- Carvalho, P. F., & Goldstone, R. L. (2014a). Effects of interleaved and blocked study on delayed test of category learning generalization. Frontiers in Psychology, 5, Article 936.
- Carvalho, P. F., & Goldstone, R. L. (2014b). Putting category learning in order: Category structure and temporal arrangement affect the benefit of interleaved over blocked study. Memory & cognition, 42, 481-495. https://doi.org/10.3758/s13421-013-0371-0
- Carvalho, P. F., & Goldstone, R. L. (2015a). The benefits of interleaved and blocked study: Different tasks benefit from different schedules of study. Psychonomic Bulletin and Review, 22(1), 281-288. https://doi.org/10.3758/s13423-014-0676-4
- Carvalho, P. F., & Goldstone, R. L. (2015b). What you learn is more than what you see: What can sequencing effects tell us about inductive category learning?. Frontiers in Psychology, 6(505), 1-12. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2015.00505
- Carvalho, P. F., & Goldstone, R. L. (2017). The sequence of study changes what information is attended to, encoded, and remembered during category learning. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 43(11), 1699-1719. https://doi.org/10.1037/xlm0000406
- Carvalho, P. F., & Goldstone, R. L. (2019). When does interleaving practice improve learning? In J. Dunlosky & K. A. Rawson (Eds.), Cambridge handbook of cognition and education (pp. 411-436). Cambridge University Press.
- Cervin-Ellqvist, M., Larsson, D., Adawi, T., Stohr, C., & Negretti, R. (2021). Metacognitive illusion or self-regulated learning? Assessing engineering students' learning strategies against the backdrop of recent advances in cognitive science. Higher Education, 82(3), 477-498. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10734-020-00635-x
- Digirolamo, G. J., & Hintzman, D. L. (1997). First impressions are lasting impressions: A primacy effect in memory for repetitions. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 4(1), 121-124. https://doi.org/10.3758/BF03210784
- Dunlosky, J., Rawson, K. A., Marsh, E. J., Nathan, M. J., & Willingham, D. T. (2013). Improving students' learning with effective learning techniques: Promising directions from cognitive and educational psychology. Psychological Science in the Public Interest, 14(1), 4-58. https://doi.org/10.1177/1529100612453266
- Foster, N. L., Mueller, M. L., Was, C., Rawson, K. A., & Dunlosky, J. (2019). Why does interleaving improve math learning? : The contributions of discriminative contrast and distributed practice. Memory and Cognition, 47(6), 1088-1101. https://doi.org/10.3758/s13421-019-00918-4
- Gleddie, A. (2021). The effects of iInterleaving on mathematical understanding. University of Alberta Libraries. https://doi.org/10.7939/r3-0amm-7287
- Kang, S. H. (2016a). Spaced repetition promotes efficient and effective learning: Policy implications for instruction. Policy Insights from the Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 3(1), 12-19. https://doi.org/10.1177/2372732215624708
- Kang, S. H. (2016b). The benefits of interleaved practice for learning. In J. C. Horvath, J. Lodge, & J. A. C. Hattie (Eds.), From the laboratory to the classroom (pp. 79-90). Routledge.
- Kang, S. H., & Pashler, H. (2012). Learning painting styles: Spacing is advantageous when it promotes discriminative contrast. Applied Cognitive Psychology, 26(1), 97-103. https://doi.org/10.1002/acp.1801
- Kost, A. S., Carvalho, P. F., & Goldstone, R. L. (2015). Can you repeat that? The effect of item repetition on interleaved and blocked study. In D. C. Noelle, R. Dale, A. S. Warlaumont, J. Yoshimi, T. Matlock, C. D. Jennings, & P. P. Maglio (eds.), Proceedings of the 37th annual meeting of the Cognitive Science Society (pp. 1189-1194). Cognitive Science Society.
- Le Blanc, K., & Simon, D. (2008). Mixed practice enhances retention and JOL accuracy for mathematical skills. Paper presented at the 49th Annual Meeting of the Psychonomic Society, Chicago, IL. November, 2008.
- Lu, X., Penney, T. B., & Kang, S. H. (2021). Category similarity affects study choices in self-regulated learning. Memory & Cognition, 49, 67-82. https://doi.org/10.3758/s13421-020-01074-w
- Mayfield, K. H., & Chase, P. N. (2002). The effects of cumulative practice on mathematics problem solving. Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, 35(2), 105-123. https://doi.org/10.1901/jaba.2002.35-105
- Rau, M. A., Aleven, V., & Rummel, N. (2013). Interleaved practice in multi-dimensional learning tasks: Which dimension should we interleave? Learning and Instruction, 23, 98-114. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.learninstruc.2012.07.003
- Roediger III, H. L., & Karpicke, J. D. (2006). The power of testing memory: Basic research and implications for educational practice. Perspectives on Psychological Science, 1(3), 181-210. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1745-6916.2006.00012.x
- Rohrer, D. (2009). Research commentary: The effects of spacing and mixing practice problems. Journal for Research in Mathematics Education, 40(1), 4-17. https://doi.org/10.5951/jresematheduc.40.1.0004
- Rohrer, D. (2012). Interleaving helps students distinguish among similar concepts. Educational Psychology Review, 24(3), 355-367. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10648-012-9201-3
- Rohrer, D., Dedrick, R. F., & Burgess, K. (2014). The benefit of interleaved mathematics practice is not limited to superficially similar kinds of problems. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 21(5), 1323-1330. https://doi.org/10.3758/s13423-014-0588-3
- Rohrer, D., Dedrick, R. F., Hartwig, M. K., & Cheung, C. N. (2020). A randomized controlled trial of interleaved mathematics practice. Journal of Educational Psychology, 112(1), 40-52. https://doi.org/10.1037/edu0000367
- Rohrer, D., Dedrick, R. F., & Stershic, S. (2015). Interleaved practice improves mathematics learning. Journal of Educational Psychology, 107(3), 900-908. https://doi.org/10.1037/edu0000001
- Rohrer, D., & Hartwig, M. K. (2020). Unanswered questions about spaced and interleaved mathematics practice. Journal of Applied Research in Memory and Cognition, 9, 433-438. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jarmac.2020.06.008
- Rohrer, D., & Taylor, K. (2007). The shuffling of mathematics problems improves learning. Instructional Science, 35(6), 481-498. https://doi.org/10.1007/s11251-007-9015-8
- Sana, F., Yan, V. X., & Kim, J. A. (2017). Study sequence matters for the inductive learning of cognitive concepts. Journal of Educational Psychology, 109(1), 84-98. https://doi.org/10.1037/edu0000119
- Taylor, K., & Rohrer, D. (2010). The effects of interleaved practice. Applied Cognitive Psychology, 24(6), 837-848. https://doi.org/10.1002/acp.1598
- Wahlheim, C. N., Dunlosky, J., & Jacoby, L. L. (2011). Spacing enhances the learning of natural concepts: An investigation of mechanisms, metacognition, and aging. Memory and Cognition, 39(5), 750-763. https://doi.org/10.3758/s13421-010-0063-y
- Warshauer, H. K. (2015). Strategies to support productive struggle. Mathematics Teaching in the Middle School, 20(7), pp. 390-393. https://doi.org/10.5951/mathteacmiddscho.20.7.0390
- Watkins, O. C., & Watkins, M. J. (1975). Buildup of proactive inhibition as a cue-overload effect. J ournal of Experimental Psychology: Human Learning and Memory, 104, 442-452. https://doi.org/10.1037/0278-7393.1.4.442
- Weltman, H. R., Timchenko, V., Sofios, H. E., Ayres, P., & Marcus, N. (2019). Evaluation of an adaptive tutorial supporting the teaching of mathematics. European Journal of Engineering Education, 44(5), 787-804. https://doi.org/10.1080/03043797.2018.1513993