참고문헌
- Barr, D. J., Levy, R., Scheepers, C., & Tily, H. J. (2013). Random effects structure for confirmatory hypothesis testing: Keep it maximal. Journal of Memory and Language, 68(3), 255-278. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jml.2012.11.001
- Bybee, J. (2001). Phonology and language use. Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press.
- Bybee, J. L. (1985). Morphology: A study of the relation between meaning and form. Philadelphia, PA: John Benjamins.
- Bybee, J. L. (2000). The phonology of the lexicon: Evidence from lexical diffusion. In M. Barlow, & S. Kemmer (Eds.), Usage-basedmodels of language (pp. 65-85). Stanford, CA: CSLI Publications.
- Dahan, D., Magnuson, J. S., & Tanenhaus, M. K. (2001). Time course of frequency effects in spoken-word recognition: Evidence from eye movements. Cognitive Psychology, 42(4), 317-367. https://doi.org/10.1006/cogp.2001.0750
- Forster, K. I., & Chambers, S. M. (1973). Lexical access and naming time. Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal Behavior, 12(6), 627-635. https://doi.org/10.1016/S0022-5371(73)80042-8
- Gahl, S. (2008). Time and thyme are not homophones: The effect of lemma frequency on word durations in spontaneous speech. Language, 84(3), 474-496. https://doi.org/10.1353/lan.0.0035
- Goldinger, S. D. (1998). Echoes of echoes? An episodic theory of lexical access. Psychological Review, 105(2), 251-279. https://doi.org/10.1037/0033-295X.105.2.251
- Goldinger, S. D., Luce, P. A., & Pisoni, D. B. (1998). Priming lexical neighbors of spoken words: Effects of competition and inhibition. Journal of Memory and Language, 28, 501-518. https://doi.org/10.1016/0749-596X(89)90009-0
- Hay, J., & Foulkes, P. (2016). The evolution of medial /t/ over real and remembered time. Language, 92(2), 298-330. https://doi.org/10.1353/lan.2016.0036
- Kilanski, K. J. (2009). The effects of token frequency and phonological neighborhood density on native and non-native English speech production (Doctoral dissertation). University of Washington, Seattle, WA.
- Koo, H. S. (2000). Characteristics of English vowels spoken by Koreans. Speech Sciences, 7(3), 99-108.
- Lee, M. (2021). The effects of lexical competitor on Korean speakers' vowel articulation (Mater's thesis). Pusan National University, Busan, Korea.
- Lindblom, B. (1968). Temporal organization of syllable production. Speech Transmission Laboratory Quarterly Progress Status Report, 9(2-3), 1-5.
- Lindblom, B. (1990). Explaining phonetic variation: A sketch of the H&H theory. In W. J. Hardcastle, & A. Marchal (Eds.), Speech production and speech modeling (pp. 403-439). Dordrecht, The Netherlands: Kluwer.
- Luce, P. A. (1986). Neighborhoods of words in the mental lexicon (Doctoral dissertation). Indiana University, Bloomington, IN.
- Luce, P. A., & Pisoni, D. B. (1998). Recognizing spoken words: The neighborhood activation model. Ear and Hearing, 19(1), 1-36. https://doi.org/10.1097/00003446-199802000-00001
- Marslen-Wilson, W. D. (1987). Functional parallelism in spoken word-recognition. Cognition, 25(1-2), 71-102. https://doi.org/10.1016/0010-0277(87)90005-9
- Marslen-Wilson, W. D. (1989). Lexical representation and process. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.
- Marslen-Wilson, W. D. (1990). Activation, competition and frequency in lexical access. In G. T. M. Altmann (Ed.), Cognitive models of speech processing: Psycholinguistic and computational perspectives (pp. 148-172). Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.
- Matuschek, H., Kliegl, R., Vasishth, S., Baayen, H., & Bates, D. (2017). Balancing type I error and power in linear mixed models. Journal of Memory and Language, 94, 305-315. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jml.2017.01.001
- McClelland, J. L., & Elman, J. L. (1986). The TRACE model of speech perception. Cognitive Psychology, 18(1), 1-86. https://doi.org/10.1016/0010-0285(86)90015-0
- Munson, B. (2001). Phonological pattern frequency and speech production in adults and children. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research, 44, 778-792. https://doi.org/10.1044/1092-4388(2001/061)
- Munson, B., & Solomon, N. P. (2004). The effect of phonological neighborhood density on vowel articulation. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research, 47(5), 1048-1058. https://doi.org/10.1044/1092-4388(2004/078)
- Norris, D. (1994). Shortlist: A connectionist model of continuous speech recognition. Cognition, 52(3), 189-234. https://doi.org/10.1016/0010-0277(94)90043-4
- Oldfield, R. C., & Wingfield, A. (1965). Response latencies in naming objects. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 17(4), 273-281. https://doi.org/10.1080/17470216508416445
- Pierrehumbert, J. B. (2001). Exemplar dynamics: Word frequency, lenition and contrast. In J. L. Bybee, & P. J. Hopper (Eds.), Frequency and the emergence of linguistic structure (pp. 137-158). Amsterdam, The Netherlands: John Benjamins.
- Pierrehumbert, J. B. (2002). Word-specific phonetics. In C. Gussenhoven, & N. Warner (Eds.), Laboratory phonology 7 (pp. 101-139). Berlin, Germany: De Gruyter Mouton.
- Pisoni, D. B., Nusbaum, H. C., Luce, P. A., & Slowiaczek, L. M. (1985). Speech perception, word recognition and the structure of the lexicon. Speech Communication, 4(1-3), 75-95. https://doi.org/10.1016/0167-6393(85)90037-8
- Ussishkin, A., & Wedel, A. (2002). Neighborhood density and the root-affix distinction. North East Linguistic Society, 32(15), 15.
- Vaden, K. I., Halpin, H. R., & Hickok, G. S. (2009). Irvine phonotactic online dictionary, version 2.0. Retrieved from http://www.iphod.com
- Wright, R. (2004). Factors of lexical competition in vowel articulation. In J. Local, R. Ogden, R. Temple, M. E. Beckman, & J. Kingston (Eds.), Papers in laboratory phonology VI: Phonetic interpretation (pp. 75-87). Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press.
- Yun, G. (2010). The effects of neighborhood density and word frequency on the production of English-learning Korean speakers. Studies in Phonetics, Phonology, and Morphology, 16(2), 217-243. https://doi.org/10.17959/SPPM.2010.16.2.217