DOI QR코드

DOI QR Code

The Effect of Dissonant Chord on Cognitive Interference and Emotion

불협화음의 종류가 인지간섭과 정서에 미치는 영향

  • 김재희 (연세대학교 인지과학 협동과정) ;
  • 한광희 (연세대학교 심리학과)
  • Received : 2021.06.09
  • Accepted : 2021.12.17
  • Published : 2022.03.31

Abstract

Many studies have shown that musical dissonance generally evokes negative affect, but few studies detail how distinct dissonant intervals, ranging in various roughness, influence emotions and cognition. This research entailed two experiments to confirm whether varying musical intervals of dissonance trigger particular negative emotions and increase cognitive interference. Experiment 1 assumed that different dissonant intervals would elicit distinct negative emotions. In a survey involving 131 participants, there was an overwhelming consensus that dissonant intervals elicited stronger high arousal negative affect than low arousal negative affect. The major 7th degree was found to evoke a significantly stronger feeling of scared. Experiment 2 investigated whether emotions-affecting dissonance would have an enhancing effect on cognitive interference. According to a color-word Stroop task conducted on 81 participants, the presence of any dissonant sound caused significantly higher reaction times and error rates than in the absence of sound. In particular, the minor 2nd degree was cognitively the most disruptive and associated with the slowest reaction times. This paper shows how different ranges of dissonance can effectively influence negative affect and heighten cognitive interference.

음악의 요소 중 불협화음은 많은 연구 결과에 따라 불쾌를 일으킨다고 여겨져 왔지만, 불협화음의 종류에 따른 인지적 역할에 대해 살펴본 연구는 희소하다. 본 연구는 불협화음의 음정 간격에 따라 특정한 정서를 일으키는지, 인지 간섭에 영향을 주는지 알아보고자 두 가지 실험을 진행하였다. 실험 1에서는 불협화음의 종류에 따라 다른 부정적 정서가 느껴질 것이라고 예상하였다. 131명을 대상으로 설문조사를 시행한 결과 불협화음은 저 각성 부적정서 보다 고 각성 부적정서를 많이 일으켰다. 특히 장 7도의 음정이 다른 불협화음보다 무섭다는 응답이 유의하게 많았다. 실험 2에서는 불협화음의 불협화 정도에 따라 인지간섭에 차등적인 영향을 미치는지 알아보고자 하였다. 81명을 대상으로 색-단어 스트룹 과제를 실시한 결과 불협화음은 모두 소리가 없는 조건에서보다 응답 시간이 느렸고, 오답률 또한 높았다. 특히 단 2도는 실험에 사용된 불협화음 중 가장 느린 응답 시간을 나타냈다. 본 연구는 불협화음의 종류가 부적정서와 인지간섭에 유의한 영향을 주는 것을 보여주었다.

Keywords

Acknowledgement

이 논문은 2019년 대한민국 교육부와 한국연구재단의 지원을 받아 수행된 연구임(NRF-2019S1A5C2A03083499).

References

  1. Athanasopoulos, G., Eerola, T., Lahdelma, I., & Kaliakatsos-Papakostas, M. (2021) Harmonic organisation conveys both universal and culture specific cues for emotional expression in music. PLoS ONE, 16(1), e0244964. DOI: 10.1371/journal.pone.0244964
  2. Bakker, D. R., & Martin, F. H. (2015). Musical chords and emotion: Major and minor triads are processed for emotion. Cognitive, Affective, & Behavioral Neuroscience, 15(1), 15-31. DOI: 10.3758/s13415-014-0309-4
  3. Bidelman, G. M. (2013). The role of the auditory brainstem in processing musically relevant pitch. Frontiers in psychology, 4, 264. DOI: 10.3389/fpsyg.2013.00264
  4. Bonin, T., & Smilek, D. (2016). Inharmonic music elicits more negative affect and interferes more with a concurrent cognitive task than does harmonic music. Attention, Perception & Psychophysics, 78(3), 946-959. DOI: 10.3758/s13414-015-1042-y
  5. Bush, G., Whalen, P. J., Rosen, B. R., Jenike, M. A., McInerney, S. C., & Rauch, S. L. (1998). The counting Stroop: An interference task specialized for functional neuroimaging-validation study with functional MRI. Human Brain Mapping, 6(4), 270-282. DOI: 10.1002/(SICI)1097-0193(1998)6:4<270::AID-HBM6>3.0.CO;2-0
  6. Chan, M., & Singhal, A. (2015). Emotion matters: Implications for distracted driving. Safety Science, 72, 302-309. DOI: 10.1016/j.ssci.2014.10.002
  7. Costa, M., Fine, P., & Ricci Bitti, P. E. (2004). Interval distributions, mode, and tonal strength of melodies as predictors of perceived emotion. Music Perception, 22(1), 1-14. DOI: 10.1525/mp.2004. 22.1.1
  8. Ekman, P. (1992). An argument for basic emotions. Cognition & Emotion, 6(3-4), 169-200. DOI: 10.1080/02699939208411068
  9. Fredrickson, B. L. (2004). The broaden-and-build theory of positive emotions. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society of London. Series B: Biological Sciences, 359(1449), 1367-1377. DOI: 10.1098/rstb.2004.1512
  10. Hargreaves, D. J., & North, A. C. (1999). The functions of music in everyday life: Redefining the social in music psychology. Psychology of Music, 27(1), 71-83. DOI: 10.1177/0305735699271007
  11. Helmholtz, H. L. (1885/1954). On the Sensations of Tone as a Physiological Basis for the Theory of Music. A.J. Ellis, Trans, Dover Publications (Ed.), New York: Cambridge University Press.
  12. Hevner, K. (1936). Experimental studies of the elements of expression in music. The American Journal of Psychology, 48(2), 246-268. DOI: 10.2307/1415746
  13. Isen, A. M., Daubman, K. A., & Nowicki, G. P. (1987). Positive affect facilitates creative problem solving. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 52(6), 1122-1131. DOI: 10.1037/0022-3514.52.6.1122
  14. Kim, S. G., Lepsien, J., Fritz, T. H., Mildner, T., & Mueller, K. (2017). Dissonance encoding in human inferior colliculus covaries with individual differences in dislike of dissonant music. Scientific Reports, 7(1), 1-10. DOI: 10.1038/s41598-017-06105-2
  15. Koelsch, S., Fritz, T., V. Cramon, D. Y., Muller, K., & Friederici, A. D. (2006). Investigating emotion with music: An fMRI study. Human Brain Mapping, 27(3), 239-250. DOI: 10.1002/hbm.20180
  16. Komeilipoor, N., Rodger, M. W., Craig, C. M., & Cesari, P. (2015). (Dis-) Harmony in movement: effects of musical dissonance on movement timing and form. Experimental Brain Research, 233(5), 1585-1595. DOI: 10.1007/s00221-015-4233-9
  17. Kwak, S. Y. (1999). Cognition of dissonance: Specifically on double pure tone, (Doctoral dissertation) Korea University, Seoul, Republic of Korea. Retrieved from http://www.riss.kr/
  18. Lahdelma, I., & Eerola, T. (2016). Single chords convey distinct emotional qualities to both naive and expert listeners. Psychology of Music, 44(1), 37-54. DOI: 10.1177/0305735614552006
  19. Lee, K. M., Skoe, E., Kraus, N., & Ashley, R. (2015). Neural transformation of dissonant intervals in the auditory brainstem. Music Perception: An Interdisciplinary Journal, 32(5), 445-459. DOI: 10.1525/mp.2015.32.5.445
  20. Lee, S. W. (2001). 협화음과 불협화음[Consonance and Dissonance], Nangman Eumak, 191-228.
  21. Masataka, N., & Perlovsky, L. (2013). Cognitive interference can be mitigated by consonant music and facilitated by dissonant music. Scientific Reports, 3(1), 1-6. DOI: 10.1038/srep02028
  22. Moser, S. J., Cutini, S., Weber, P., & Schroeter, M. L. (2009). Right prefrontal brain activation due to Stroop interference is altered in attention-deficit hyperactivity disorder-a functional near-infrared spectroscopy study. Psychiatry Research: Neuroimaging, 173(3), 190-195. DOI: 10.1016/j.pscychresns.2008.10.003
  23. Pallesen, K. J., Brattico, E., Bailey, C., Korvenoja, A., Koivisto, J., Gjedde, A., & Carlson, S. (2005). Emotion processing of major, minor, and dissonant chords: A functional magnetic resonance imaging study. Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences, 1060(1), 450-453. DOI: 10.1196/annals.1360.047
  24. Pereira, A. G., Cruz, A., Lima, S. Q., & Moita, M. A. (2012). Silence resulting from the cessation of movement signals danger. Current Biology, 22(16), R627-R628. DOI: 10.1016/j.cub.2012.06.015
  25. Plomp, R., & Levelt, W. J. M. (1965). Tonal consonance and critical bandwidth. The Journal of the Acoustical Society of America, 38(4), 548-560. DOI: 10.1121/1.1909741
  26. Russell, J. A. (1980). A circumplex model of affect. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 39(6), 1161. DOI: 10.1037/h0077714
  27. Salters-Pedneault, K., Gentes, E., & Roemer, L. (2007). The role of fear of emotion in distress, arousal, and cognitive interference following an emotional stimulus. Cognitive Behaviour Therapy, 36(1), 12-22. DOI: 10.1080/16506070600874281
  28. Sammler, D., Grigutsch, M., Fritz, T., & Koelsch, S. (2007). Music and emotion: Electrophysiological correlates of the processing of pleasant and unpleasant music. Psychophysiology, 44(2), 293-304. DOI: 10.1111/j.1469-8986.2007.00497.x
  29. Sanada, M., Kuwamoto, T., & Katayama, J. I. (2021). Deviant consonance and dissonance capture attention differently only when task demand is high: An ERP study with three-stimulus oddball paradigm. International Journal of Psychophysiology, 166, 1-8. DOI: 10.1016/j.ijpsycho.2021.04.008
  30. Schroeter, M. L., Zysset, S., Wahl, M., & von Cramon, D. Y. (2004). Prefrontal activation due to Stroop interference increases during development-an event-related fNIRS study. Neuroimage, 23(4), 1317-1325. DOI: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2004.08.001
  31. Sloboda, J. A., & Juslin, P. N. (2001). Psychological perspectives on music and emotion. In P. N. Juslin & J. A. Sloboda (Eds.), Music and emotion: Theory and research, Oxford University Press.
  32. Song, M. K. (2018), 연주자를 위한 조성 음악 분석 2[Analysis of tonal music for musical players 2], Yesol, Seoul.
  33. Storbeck, J., & Clore, G. L. (2007). On the interdependence of cognition and emotion. Cognition and Emotion, 21(6), 1212-1237. DOI: 10.1080/ 02699930701438020
  34. Watson, D., & Tellegen, A. (1985). Toward a consensual structure of mood. Psychological Bulletin, 98(2), 219. DOI: 10.1037/0033-2909.98.2.219
  35. Watson, D., Clark, L. A., & Tellegen, A. (1988). Development and validation of brief measures of positive and negative affect: the PANAS scales. Journal of personality and social psychology, 54(6), 1063. DOI: 10.1037/0022-3514.54.6.1063
  36. Watson, D. (1988). The vicissitudes of mood measurement: effects of varying descriptors, time frames, and response formats on measures of positive and negative affect. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 55(1), 128. DOI: 10.1037/0022-3514.55.1.128