DOI QR코드

DOI QR Code

The effect of lyrical and non-lyrical background music on different types of language processing - An ERP study

배경음악 및 가사가 실시간 언어처리에 미치는 영향 - 사건 관련 전위 연구

  • Received : 2020.12.17
  • Accepted : 2020.12.17
  • Published : 2020.12.31

Abstract

People read in different settings, including when music is playing in the background. Whether the presence of music facilitates, hinders, or does not affect language processing is an on-going debate in the current literature. The present study used ERPs to examine the influence of music on orthographic, semantic, and syntactic processing by inspecting P2, N400, and P600 responses, respectively. A total of 60 participants judged the correctness of visually presented sentences while listening to music with lyrics, music without lyrics, or in silence. The results showed that the P2 and P600 effects were larger in the silent condition than in the music-with-lyrics condition, while there were no N400 differences among the conditions. This indicates that only lyrical music interferes with orthographic and syntactic language processing, while it has no, or minimal, effect on semantic processing. The results are discussed in relation to the background music interference effect.

우리는 사람들이 음악을 들으면서 책을 읽거나 학습활동을 하는 경우를 자주 발견하게 된다. 이러한 배경음악이 언어처리 과정에 어떠한 영향을 미치는지에 대해 그동안 활발한 논의가 진행됐다. 하지만 음악의 언어처리에 대한 영향성은 아직까지 명확히 밝혀져 있지 않은 상황에 있다. 본 연구는 사건관련전위(ERP)를 활용하여, 각 언어의 처리 과정-표기 정보 처리, 의미 정보 처리, 통사 정보 처리에 따라 나타나는 배경음악의 효과를 분석하고 이를 통해 음악이 언어처리에 미치는 영향성을 규명하고자 하였다. 총 60명의 피험자를 세 가지 실험조건그룹(가사 있는 음악 조건, 가사 없는 음악 조건, 무 음악 조건)으로 구분하고, 각 언어처리 과정에 해당하는 문장 자극을 제시하고 이에 대한 뇌파를 측정하였다. 실험 결과, 무음악 조건에 비해 가사 있는 음악 조건에서 표기 처리를 반영하는 P2와 통사 처리를 반영하는 P600의 진폭이 유의미하게 낮게 나타났다. 이에 반해, 의미 처리 과정을 반영하는 N400은 조건 간의 차이가 발견되지 않았다. 이러한 결과는 가사 있는 음악이 표기 처리 과정과 통사 처리 과정을 방해하는 한편, 의미 처리 과정에는 영향을 미치지 않는다는 점을 시사한다.

Keywords

References

  1. Anderson, S. A., & Fuller, G. B. (2010). Effect of music on reading comprehension of junior high school students. School Psychology Quarterly, 25(3), 178. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0021213
  2. Barber, H., Vergara, M., & Carreiras, M. (2004). Syllable-frequency effects in visual word recognition: evidence from ERPs. Neuroreport, 15(3), 545-548. https://doi.org/10.1097/00001756-200403010-00032
  3. Besson, M., Faita, F., Peretz, I., Bonnel, A.-M., & Requin, J. (1998). Singing in the Brain: Independence of Lyrics and Tunes. Psychological Science, 9(6), 494-498. https://doi.org/10.1111/1467-9280.00091
  4. Brandt, A. K., Slevc, R., & Gebrian, M. (2012). Music and early language acquisition. Frontiers in psychology, 3, 327. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2012.00327
  5. Carreiras, M., Vergara, M., & Barber, H. (2005). Early event-related potential effects of syllabic processing during visual word recognition. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 17(11), 1803-1817. https://doi.org/10.1162/089892905774589217
  6. Cauchard, F., Cane, J. E., & Weger, U. W. (2012). Influence of background speech and music in interrupted reading: An eye tracking study. Applied Cognitive Psychology, 26(3), 381-390. https://doi.org/10.1002/acp.1837
  7. Chien, P. J., & Chan, S. H. (2015). Old songs can be as fresh as new: an ERP study on lyrics processing. Journal of Neurolinguistics, 35, 55-67. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jneuroling.2015.02.002
  8. Christoffels, I. K., Ganushchak, L., & Koester, D. (2013). Language conflict in translation: An ERP study of translation production. Journal of Cognitive Psychology, 25(5), 646-664. https://doi.org/10.1080/20445911.2013.821127
  9. Costa, A., Strijkers, K., Martin, C., & Thierry, G. (2009). The time course of word retrieval revealed by event-related brain potentials during overt speech. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 106(50), 21442-21446. https://doi.org/10.1073/pnas.0908921106
  10. Daltrozzo, J., & Schon, D. (2009). Conceptual processing in music as revealed by N400 effects on words and musical targets. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 21(10), 1882-1892. https://doi.org/10.1162/jocn.2009.21113
  11. Daltrozzo, J., Tillmann, B., Platel, H., & Schon, D. (2010). Temporal aspects of the feeling of familiarity for music and the emergence of conceptual processing. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 22(8), 1754-1769. https://doi.org/10.1162/jocn.2009.21311
  12. Dittinger, E., Barbaroux, M., D'Imperio, M., Jäncke, L., Elmer, S., & Besson, M. (2016). Professional music training and novel word learning: From faster semantic encoding to longer-lasting word representations. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 28(10), 1584-1602. https://doi.org/10.1162/jocn_a_00997
  13. Featherstone, C. R., Morrison, C. M., Waterman, M. G., & MacGregor, L. J. (2013). Semantics, syntax or neither? A case for resolution in the interpretation of N500 and P600 responses to harmonic incongruities. PloS one, 8(11), e76600. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0076600
  14. Fitzroy, A. B., & Sanders, L. D. (2012). Musical expertise modulates early processing of syntactic violations in language. Frontiers in psychology, 3, 603. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2012.00603
  15. Freeburne, C. M., & Fleischer, M. S. (1952). The effect of music distraction upon reading rate and comprehension. Journal of Educational Psychology, 43(2), 101-109. https://doi.org/10.1037/h0054219
  16. Friederici, A. D. (2002). Towards a neural basis of auditory sentence processing. Trends in cognitive sciences, 6(2), 78-84. https://doi.org/10.1016/S1364-6613(00)01839-8
  17. Gordon, R. L., Schon, D., Magne, C., Astésano, C., & Besson, M. (2010). Words and melody are intertwined in perception of sung words: EEG and behavioral evidence. PLoS One, 5(3), e9889. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0009889
  18. Hall, J. C. (1952). The effect of background music on the reading comprehension of 278 eighth and ninth grade students. The Journal of Educational Research, 45(6), 451-458. https://doi.org/10.1080/00220671.1952.10881962
  19. Herring, D., & Scott, J. (2018). The effect of lyrical and instrumental music on reading comprehension tasks. Journal of Emerging Investigators, 1-6.
  20. Hoeks, J. C., Stowe, L. A., & Doedens, G. (2004). Seeing words in context: the interaction of lexical and sentence level information during reading. Cognitive Brain Research, 19(1), 59-73. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cogbrainres.2003.10.022
  21. Holcomb, P. J., & Grainger, J. (2006). On the time course of visual word recognition: An event-related potential investigation using masked repetition priming. Journal of cognitive neuroscience, 18(10), 1631-1643. https://doi.org/10.1162/jocn.2006.18.10.1631
  22. Hughes, R. W. (2014). Auditory distraction: A duplex mechanism account. PsyCh Journal, 3(1), 30-41. https://doi.org/10.1002/pchj.44
  23. Kallinen, K. (2002). Reading news from a pocket computer in a distracting environment: effects of the tempo of background music. Computers in Human Behavior, 18(5), 537-551. https://doi.org/10.1016/S0747-5632(02)00005-5
  24. Kiger, D. M. (1989). Effects of music information load on a reading comprehension task. Perceptual and motor skills, 69(2), 531-534. https://doi.org/10.2466/pms.1989.69.2.531
  25. Koelsch, S., Kasper, E., Sammler, D., Schulze, K., Gunter, T., & Friederici, A. D. (2004). Music, language and meaning: brain signatures of semantic processing. Nature neuroscience, 7(3), 302. https://doi.org/10.1038/nn1197
  26. Kong, L., Zhang, B., Zhang, J. X., & Kang, C. (2012). P200 can be modulated by orthography alone in reading Chinese words. Neuroscience letters, 529(2), 161-165. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neulet.2012.09.028
  27. Kutas, M., & Federmeier, K. D. (2011). Thirty years and counting: finding meaning in the N400 component of the event-related brain potential (ERP). Annual Review of Psychology, 62, 621-647. https://doi.org/10.1146/annurev.psych.093008.131123
  28. Maess, B., Koelsch, S., Gunter, T. C., & Friederici, A. D. (2001). Musical syntax is processed in Broca's area: an MEG study. Nature neuroscience, 4(5), 540-545. https://doi.org/10.1038/87502
  29. Marsh, J. E., & Jones, D. M. (2010). Cross-modal distraction by background speech: What role for meaning?. Noise and Health, 12(49), 210. https://doi.org/10.4103/1463-1741.70499
  30. Marsh, J. E., Hughes, R. W., & Jones, D. M. (2008). Auditory distraction in semantic memory: A process-based approach. Journal of Memory and Language, 58(3), 682-700. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jml.2007.05.002
  31. Marsh, J. E., Hughes, R. W., & Jones, D. M. (2009). Interference by process, not content, determines semantic auditory distraction. Cognition, 110(1), 23-38. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cognition.2008.08.003
  32. Martin, R. C., Wogalter, M. S., & Forlano, J. G. (1988). Reading comprehension in the presence of unattended speech and music. Journal of memory and language, 27(4), 382-398. https://doi.org/10.1016/0749-596X(88)90063-0
  33. Miller, L. R. (1947). Some effects of radio-listening on the efficiency of reading-type study activities. Journal of educational psychology, 38(2), 105. https://doi.org/10.1037/h0062228
  34. Mitchell, A. H. (1949). The effect of radio programs on silent reading achievement of ninety-one sixth grade students. The Journal of Educational Research, 42(6), 460-470. https://doi.org/10.1080/00220671.1949.10881709
  35. Mitra, P., & Coch, D. (2009). A masked priming ERP study of letter processing using single letters and false fonts. Cognitive, Affective, & Behavioral Neuroscience, 9(2), 216-228. https://doi.org/10.3758/CABN.9.2.216
  36. Patel, A. D., Gibson, E., Ratner, J., Besson, M., & Holcomb, P. J. (1998). Processing syntactic relations in language and music: An event-related potential study. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 10(6), 717-733. https://doi.org/10.1162/089892998563121
  37. Perham, N., & Currie, H. (2014). Does listening to preferred music improve reading comprehension performance?. Applied Cognitive Psychology, 28(2), 279-284. https://doi.org/10.1002/acp.2994
  38. Ransdell, S. E., & Gilroy, L. (2001). The effects of background music on word processed writing. Computers in Human Behavior, 17(2), 141-148. https://doi.org/10.1016/S0747-5632(00)00043-1
  39. Saito, Y., Ishii, K., Sakuma, N., Kawasaki, K., Oda, K., & Mizusawa, H. (2012). Neural substrates for semantic memory of familiar songs: is there an interface between lyrics and melodies?. PloS one, 7(9), e46354. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0046354
  40. Sammler, D., Koelsch, S., & Friederici, A. D. (2011). Are left fronto-temporal brain areas a prerequisite for normal music-syntactic processing?. Cortex, 47(6), 659-673. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cortex.2010.04.007
  41. Samson, S., & Zatorre, R. J. (1991). Recognition memory for text and melody of songs after unilateral temporal lobe lesion: Evidence for dual encoding. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 17(4), 793-804. https://doi.org/10.1037//0278-7393.17.4.793
  42. Serafine, M. L., Crowder, R. G., & Repp, B. H. (1984). Integration of melody and text in memory for songs. Cognition, 16(3), 285-303. https://doi.org/10.1016/0010-0277(84)90031-3
  43. Schon, D., Boyer, M., Moreno, S., Besson, M., Peretz, I., & Kolinsky, R. (2008). Songs as an aid for language acquisition. Cognition, 106(2), 975-983. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cognition.2007.03.005
  44. Steinbeis, N., & Koelsch, S. (2007). Shared neural resources between music and language indicate semantic processing of musical tension-resolution patterns. Cerebral Cortex, 18(5), 1169-1178. https://doi.org/10.1093/cercor/bhm149
  45. Thompson, W. F., Schellenberg, E. G., & Letnic, A. K. (2012). Fast and loud background music disrupts reading comprehension. Psychology of Music, 40(6), 700-708. https://doi.org/10.1177/0305735611400173
  46. Vasilev, M. R., Kirkby, J. A., & Angele, B. (2018). Auditory distraction during reading: A Bayesian meta-analysis of a continuing controversy. Perspectives on Psychological Science, 13(5), 567-597. https://doi.org/10.1177/1745691617747398