DOI QR코드

DOI QR Code

Loving or Eating?: Eating Meat and Mind Perception toward Animals and Sexually Objectified Women

사랑할까, 먹을까?: 동물과 성적 객체화된 여성에 대한 마음지각과 고기를 먹는 행동의 관계

  • Received : 2019.05.27
  • Accepted : 2019.06.26
  • Published : 2019.06.30

Abstract

Do animals have a mind? Our understanding about whether animals have minds depends on our relationship with animals, as we cannot determine animals' actual minds. These two studies presented here thus examined the meat paradox, that is, an inconsistency between love for animals and the act of enjoying eating meat in the context of mind perception. Study 1 examined whether mind perceptions toward various animals are classified on the basis of experience-related capacities, such as feeling pain, and agency-related capacities, such as having self-control. In Study 2, mind perceptions toward cows and sexually objectified women were classified on the basis of food condition and non-food condition. In the food condition (experimental condition), cows were portrayed as products for meat consumption, whereas in the control condition, they were described as animals living on a farm, eating grass. The results of Study 2 demonstrated revealed that mind perception was positively associated with how morally incorrect it was to eat animals. Study 2 thus demonstrated that the scores of mind perception toward cows and sexually objectified women in the experimental condition were significantly lower than those in the control condition. These reduced mind attribution in the experimental condition implied that people may be motivated to reduce cognitive dissonance between their attitudes toward animals, such as loving them, and their behaviors, such as, eating meat. In addition, these results suggest that objectification toward animals may impact the objectification and mind perception toward human beings as well. These findings highlight the role of dissonance reduction in the meat paradox and objectification theory so as to understand basic psychological processes involved while making moral choices in everyday life.

동물은 마음을 갖고 있는가? 우리는 동물의 마음을 실제로 잘 알 수 없기 때문에 동물과의 관계를 통해 동물의 마음을 추정할 가능성이 높다. 본 연구에서는 동물을 사랑하는 태도와 고기를 먹는 행동에 대한 모순적 관계를 동물에 대한 마음지각의 맥락에서 두 개의 연구를 통해 검증하였다. 연구 1에서는 다양한 동물에 대한 마음지각을 경험의 차원과 주도성의 차원에서 분석하였다. 연구 2에서는 소를 고기로 기술한 실험조건과 고기가 아닌 동물로 기술한 통제조건에서 소와 성적 객체화된 여성에 대한 마음지각을 각각 측정하였다. 그 결과 연구 1에서는 동물에 대한 마음지각의 정도가 높을수록 고기를 먹는 행동을 도덕적으로 옳지 않은 것으로 판단하는 평정점수가 높았다. 연구 2에서는 소를 고기로 기술한 실험조건에서 통제조건보다 소와 성적 객체화된 여성에 대한 마음지각이 통제조건보다 낮게 나타났다. 따라서 동물이 '고기'로 제시되는 상황에서는 동물에 대한 마음지각의 조정을 통해 동물을 해치는 행동에 대한 심리적 불편감을 감소시킬 가능성을 생각해볼 수 있다. 이 결과는 인지부조화이론에 따라 고기를 먹는 행동과 동물에 대한 태도의 차이에서 발생하는 심리적 불편감을 줄이고자 동물에 대한 마음지각을 조정하는 것으로 해석할 수 있다. 또한 동물에 대한 객체화는 인간에 대한 객체화 및 마음지각에 영향을 끼침을 추정해볼 수 있다. 이 결과는 마음지각, 인지부조화이론 및 객체화이론이 일상에서의 도덕성과 연관된 태도와 행동의 모순적 관계를 설명하는데 유용함을 보여준다.

Keywords

References

  1. 김영환 (2018. 8. 11). 맹자가 말한 개를 먹지 말아야 할 철학적 이유. 한겨레신문. http://www.hani.co.kr/arti/animalpeople/human_animal/857162.html
  2. 서유진 (2019.1.22). 한국인 식성 변화, 곡물.채소${\downarrow}$ 과일.고기${\uparrow}$. 중앙일보. https://news.joins.com/article/23311217
  3. 신소윤 (2018.12.30). 고기를 먹는다는 건, 취향의 문제를 넘어서는 일. 한겨레신문. http://www.hani.co.kr/arti/animalpeople/human_animal/876293.html
  4. 윤승민 (2016. 4. 15). 한국인 1인, 1년간 먹는 육류량 51.3 kg. 경향신문. http://news.khan.co.kr/kh_news/khan_art_view.html
  5. Bandura, A. (1999). Moral disengagement in the perpetration of inhumanities. Personality and Social Psychology Review, 3, 193-209. DOI: org/10.1207/s15327957pspr0303_3
  6. Bastian, B., Costello, K., Loughnan, S., & Hodson, G. (2012). When closing the human-animal divide expands moral concern. Social Psychological and Personality Science, 36, 100-107. DOI: org/10.1177/1948550611425106
  7. Bastian, B., Loughhan, S., Haslam, N., & Radke, H. (2012). Don't mind meat? The denial of mind to animals used for human consumption. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 38, 247-256. DOI: 10.1177/0146167211424291
  8. Bevens, C. L., Brown, A., L., & Loughnan, S. (2018). The role of self-objectification and women's blame, sympathy, and support for a rape victim. PLoS ONE, 13, e0199808. DOI: org/10.1371/journal.pone.0199808
  9. Cikara, M., Eberhardt, J. L., Fiske, S. T. (2010). From agents to objects: Sexist attitudes and neural responses to sexualized targets. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 23, 540-551. DOI: 10.1162/jocn.2010.21497
  10. Epley, N., & Waytz, A. (2009). Mind perception. In S. T. Fiske, D. T. Gilbert, & G. Lindsay (Eds.), The handbook of social psychology (5th ed., pp. 498-541). New York, NY: Wiley.
  11. Epstein, R. A. (2002). Animals as Objects, or Subjects, of Rights. University Chicago Law & Economics, Olin Working Paper, 171. DOI: 10.2139/ssrn.359240
  12. Festinger, L. (1957). A theory of cognitive dissonance. Stanford, CA: Stanford University Press.
  13. Fiske, S. T., Cuddy, A. J. C., & Glick, P. (2007). Universal dimensions of social cognition: Warmth and competence. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 11, 77-83. DOI: org/10.1016/j.tics.2006.11.005
  14. Foer, J. S. (2009). Eating animals. New York, NY: Little, Brown.
  15. Haslam, N., Bain, P., Douge, L., Lee, M.,& Bastian, B. (2005). More human than you: Attributing humanness to self and others. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 89, 973-950. DOI: 10.1037/0022-3514.89.6.937
  16. Harris & Fiske (2006). Dehumanizing the lowest of the low: Neuro-imaging responses to extreme outgroups. Psychological Science, 17, 847-853. DOI: 10.1111/j.1467-9280.2006.01793.x
  17. Haslam, N. (2006). Dehumanization: An integrative review. Personality and Social Psychology Review, 10, 252-264. DOI: org/10.1207/s15327957pspr1003_4
  18. Haslam, N., Loughnan, S., Kashima, Y., & Bain, P. (2008). Attributing and denying humanness to others. European Review of Social Psychology, 19, 55-65. DOI: org/10.1080/10463280801981645
  19. Haslam, N., Kashima, Y., Loughnan, S., Shi, J., & Suitner, C. (2008). Subhuman, inhuman, and superhuman: Contrasting humans with nonhumans in three cultures. Social Cognition, 26, 248-258. DOI: org/10.1521/soco.2008.26.2.248
  20. Hoogland, C., de Boer, J., & Boersema, J. (2005). Transparency of the meat chain in the light of food culture and history. Appetite, 45, 15-23. DOI: 10.1016/j.appet.2005.01.010
  21. Fredrickson, B. L., & Roberts, T.-A. (1997). Objectification theory: Toward understanding women's lived experiences and mental health risks. Psychology of Women Quarterly, 21, 173-206. DOI: 10.1111/j.1471-6402.1997.tb00108.x
  22. Gray, K., Young, L., & Waytz, A. (2012) Mind perception is the essence of morality. Psychological Inquiry, 23, 101-124. DOI: 10.1080/1047840X.2012.651387
  23. Gray. K., Knobe, J., Sheskin, M., Bllom, P., & Barett, L. F. (2011). More than a body: Mind perception and the nature of objectification. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 101, 1207-1220. DOI: 10.1037/a0025883
  24. Gray, H. M., Gray, K., & Wegner, D. M. (2007). Dimensions of mind perception. Science, 315, 619. DOI: 10.1126/science.1134475
  25. Jahoda, G. (1999). Images of savages: Ancient roots of modern prejudice in western culture. London: Rouledge & Kegan Paul.
  26. Kozak, M., Marsch, A., & Wegner, D. (2006). What do I think you're doing? Action identification and mind attribution. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 90, 543-555. DOI: org/10.1002/ejsp.755
  27. Loughnan, S., Haslam, N., Murnane, T., Vaes, J., Reynolds, C., & Suitner, C. (2010). Objectification leads to depersonalization: The denial of mind and moral concern to objectified others. European Journal of Social Psychology, 40, 709-717. DOI: org/10.1002/ejsp.755
  28. Puvia, E., & Vaes, J. (2013). Being a body: Women's appearance related self-views and their dehumanization of sexually objectified female targets. Sex Roles, 68, 484-495. DOI: 10.1007/s11199-012-0255-y
  29. Son, E. (2006). The influence of objectification experiences on Women's mental health. The Korean Journal of Woman Psychology, 11, 399-417.