DOI QR코드

DOI QR Code

Why Learners Found Transfer Pricing Difficult? Implications for Directors

  • Abeysekera, Indra (College of Business and Law, Charles Darwin University) ;
  • Jebeile, Sam (ANU College of Business and Economics, Australian National University)
  • Received : 2018.10.20
  • Accepted : 2018.12.23
  • Published : 2019.02.28

Abstract

A recent survey of Australian directors conducted by the Financial Reporting Council found that directors require a detailed understanding of technical accounting issues. With the aim of understanding learner difficulties in learning and applying higher learning material relevant to directors, this study explores the transfer pricing topic taught as a case presentation in an undergraduate accounting program at an Australian university. Before intervention with improvements, this study invited 25 students to take part in the study after they had learned the topic and been given one week to understand it. By adopting a transfer pricing problem presented in their essential reading and interviewing those students to gain further insights, the study found that learners experienced conceptual difficulties at various stages in attempting to learn. Intervention to ease learning difficulties was addressed through instructor training. The intervention improvements included using guided workbooks to develop a better understanding of concepts among learners, and representing the problem at hand with diagrams. After intervention with improvements, this study repeated the same procedures with 25 students who had not taken part in the previous study and found that interventions increased the learning. Results have implications for most directors, who are novices to the detailed technical accounting issues of transfer pricing.

Keywords

1. Introduction

A survey conducted by the Financial Reporting Council in Australia to determine the financial literacy of Australian directors asked financial professionals about directors’ financial literacy and found that these professionals rated directors’ knowledge about more technical accounting issues as less than adequate (CAANZ, 2016). This is a concern because directors require a quite detailed understanding of technical accounting issues such as transfer pricing in making sound decisions affecting organisational economics, efficiency, and effectiveness.

Transfer pricing has to do with setting the price of goods and services sold within multi-divisional, multi-office, or multi-national firms. The efficient setting of transfer pricing allows each division, office, or firm within a group of firms, to function as a profit centre with the aim of maximising overall profits of the enterprise. An enterprise can also set transfer price to transfer profits from a high-tax jurisdiction country to a low-tax jurisdiction country. Governments lose tax revenue from companies due to transfer price arrangements and have established various rules a firm should follow in setting a transfer price for cross-border transactions within a group of firms. Transfer pricing is a challenging issue for directors because setting inappropriate transfer prices can increase government scrutiny and potential prosecution (Power, 2012).

Knowledge about transfer pricing requires understanding a nexus of accounting concepts and their application to determine the appropriate transfer price. Hence, learners have reported that transfer pricing is a difficult to topic to understand. This difficulty becomes evident from students’ consistently poor examination performance on this question.

Transfer pricing decides the selling price for transactions of goods and services between divisions within a firm. Firms can label their divisions as investment centres or profit centres. This labelling allows directors to evaluate profits or investments made by the divisions. Because directors measure how well divisions perform by profits and investments made, each division can tend to increase its profits and investments while ignoring how this influences the firm. Transfer prices are internal selling prices; they form revenue for the selling division that increases profits, and cost to the buying division that decreases profits (LangfieldSmith, Thorne, & Hilton, 2006). The divisions measure the financial performance using some measure of profit.

This study explored why students have difficulties in understanding transfer pricing, a topic taught in the management accounting course unit of the undergraduate course at an Australian university. Investigating student learning difficulties in the transfer pricing topic helps us to understand difficulties facing novice students in learning topics of higher-order thinking which demand a sound understanding of lower-level thinking. Many directors who do not have a strong accounting background and are new to how transfer pricing works will also experience similar difficulties and become equivalent to novice-students of transfer pricing.

This study presents how we overcame student difficulties in learning transfer pricing. The next section outlines cognitive load theory and the relevant literature. Section 3 reports the data collection including problem presentation. Section 4 presents results and discussion, as well as implications for teaching transfer pricing to directors.

2. Theory and Literature Review

Cognitive load theory states that instructional design must guide learners to discover and build knowledge. It explains the association between knowledge and learning, and how instructional design can help to foster the building of knowledge (Moreno & Park, 2010). Learning requires exerting cognitive load, but instructional design can moderate this load. Cognitive load arises from consuming the working memory or short-term memory in learning new information. According to cognitive load theory, the human cognitive architecture has a limited short-term memory and a large long-term memory (Kahneman, 1973; Miller, 1956). The short-term memory makes meaning out of information by thinking, and long-term memory stores information for later use (Schneider & Schiffrin, 1977). A role of learning is to transfer an organised pattern of thought or behaviour (schema) developed in the working (short-term) memory to long-term memory so that the learner becomes familiar with finding solutions to any similar problems. The transfer of an organised pattern or thought from working memory to longterm memory frees up working memory for further learning (Sweller, 1994). Novices use more working memory because they lack previously formed thought patterns and behaviours relevant to solving a given task and therefore they must think more to solve the task. Experts use less working memory because they can access solutions to the problem by calling on previously formed thoughts and behavioural patterns deposited in long-term memory (Sweller, Ayers, & Kalyuga, 2011, p. 21).

For learning to occur, the cognitive load imposed by the task should not exceed the total working memory available. Three types of additive cognitive load (intrinsic load, extraneous load, and germane load) impose the burden on working memory. Intrinsic load arises from inherent difficulty of a task that requires interrelating different ideas. Extraneous load arises from poor design of the teaching methods used in helping students to learn a task. Germane load arises from the learner effort exerted on the task (Sweller & Chandler, 1994, p. 192).

Although previous studies agree that instructional design can decrease the cognitive load, mixed findings exist for intrinsic cognitive load. Some findings suggest that instructional design can decrease the intrinsic cognitive load, by presenting items of information (that is, elements) sequentially, and combining similar items in the instruction (Lee, Plass, & Homer, 2006; Pollock, Chandler, & Sweller, 2002; van Merrienboer, Kirschner, & Kester, 2003).

Learning various topics can invoke different types of thoughts and behavioural patterns (that is, schema). Some topics require building a simple schema, whereas for others a complex schema must be built. Learning accounting requires building a complex schema (Blaney, Kalyuga, & Sweller, 2010). Learning accounting topics requires learning many new concepts and formulas (that is, elements) and simultaneously combining them. The complexities lead to students using too much of their working memory, and this can decrease learners’ capacity to build a schema to increase learning (Sithole, Chandler, Abeysekera, & Paas, 2017). The transfer pricing topic is a case in point, where students need to learn and simultaneously combine new ideas and formulas to build from a basic to a complex schema.

Figure 1 summarises five order levels of the understandings the learner must develop in order to learn transfer pricing. The first order shows the basic concepts in transfer pricing. The second-order concepts build upon the first-order concepts. It is essential that students understand the first-order concepts, to understand the second-order concepts. Each higher order level builds upon the understanding of lower-level concepts and understanding the fifth-ordder concepts requires understanding all other lower-level concepts. Solving a problem about transfer price requires decision-makers to simultaneously process information relating to all order levels. It is essential to understand the conceptual difficulties experienced by learners, to ease learning difficulties in mastering transfer pricing. We assessed learner's written answers against the marking guide prepared at the time of formulating the problem. We conducted interviews while students were attempting the problem, to understand the learning difficulties they experienced.

OTGHEU_2019_v6n1_9_f0001.png 이미지

Figure 1: Transfer price  calculation

We stated and tested the alternate hypotheses according to order-level mastery, and expected that imporvement-interventions diagnosed from the pre-intervention stage would improve the learning at the post-intervention stage. The learning was measured by assessing scores against the marking guides, and statistical significance is measured using Pearson Chi-Square (goodness-of-fit) test at 5 percent significance level.

H1: Interventions imporve learning at first-order and second-order levels (Part A of the problem).

H2: Interventions imporve learning at third-order and fourth-order levels (Part B of the problem).

H3: Interventions imporve learning at fifth-order level (Part C of the problem).

3. Methodology

This study was conducted in three phases (Figure 2).

OTGHEU_2019_v6n1_9_f0001.png 이미지

Figure 2: Research design

3.1. Phase 1: Problem Design

The first phase, pre-intervention, investigated learner difficulties in leaming transfer pricing. Phase 2 conducted a workshop for instructors to inform about learner challenges and develop strategies to overcome them. Phase 3, the post-intervention phase, implemented the agreed strategies to overcome learning difficulties to a future cohort of learners.

Students were invited to take part in the research study by advertising it in tutorial classes as a research project examining learning difficulties in transfer pricing, according to the ethics agreement. A researcher randomly selected 25 students from those who expressed interest in taking part. Students enrolled in this course unit had attained a 56 percent average mark and had an average 1.94 Grade Point Average. The t-tests confirmed that these averages were not statistically different from the total average mark and GPA of students of this management accounting course unit, establishing their representativeness of the class. The study used a common teaching instructor for all students to eliminate the effect of instructor difference. Students had completed the transfer pricing topic before the mid-term break and we invited them to take part in the study in the first week after the break. We gave students a typical problem they had faced in leaming transfer pricing, from their essential reading textbook written by Langfield-Smith et al. (2006) (Table 1). The problem had three parts to be answered as Part A, Part B, and Part C.

Table 1: Transfer Pricing Problem

OTGHEU_2019_v6n1_9_t0001.png 이미지

Spike Sports has several divisions. However, only one division tr ansfers products to another internal division. The Polyfabric Divis ion produces a special fabric, Sweatless, which is then transferre d to the Sportswear Division. Each unit of the Sweatless fabric is further processed to make tracksuits (each unit of Sweatless fa bric makes one tracksuit) by the Sportswear Division, and the tra cksuits are sold to customers at the list price of \($\)250 per unit. Un der the existing internal agreement, the Polyfabric Division can o nly sell to external customers after they have supplied the Sports wear Division with all of their requirements. The existing agreem ent also stipulates that the Sweatless fabric must be supplied to t he Sportswear Division at standard manufacturing cost plus 10 p er cent. Assume that unlimited quantities of Sweatless fabric can be purchased and sold on the open market at \($\)100 per unit.

The following table shows the detailed standard unit cost structure for each division:

^ Not including transfer price for Sweatless fabric.

* Manufacturing overhead cost in the Polyfabric Division is 50% fixed and 50% variable.

** Manufacturing overhead cost in the Sportswear Division is 60% fixed and 40% variable.

Required:

(Part A) What is the contribution margin per unit for each division under the existing transfer pricing agreement?

(Part B) Assume that the existing transfer pricing agreement was cancelled and the managers of each division could act autonomously (including buying and selling on the open market). Use the general transfer pricing rule to calculate the lowest transfer price per unit of Sweatless that would be acceptable to the Polyfabric Division.

(Part C) Assume that there is excess capacity in the Sportswear Division and assume that the Sportswear Division has received a special order from the Australian Soccer Team (Socceroos) urgently requiring 100 tracksuits at $200 each for their World Cup campaign in Germany. The Public Relations department has directed the Sportswear Division to accept the special order. From the perspective of the Spike Sports company as a whole, do you agree with the decision to accept the special order? Justify your answer.

3.1.1. Part A of the Presented Learning Problem

Part A required problem solving at the first-order and second-order levels. In Part A, students conducted the activities in the following order.

(i) Calculated the contribution margin (difference between selling price and variable costs) from each unit produced, for each of the divisions, based on the transfer pricing rules of the firm. Part A also required students to know the meaning of contribution margin as the difference between selling price and variable costs. They were required to know that variable cost is adding direct material, direct labour, and variable overhead. This transfer pricing problem needed students to make four separate calculations.

(ii) Calculated selling prices for the two divisions in the firm: Division A and Division B.

(iii) Calculated total variable costs for the two divisions separately. (iv) Included the transfer price of Division A, as a buying cost in Division B.

(v) Calculated contribution margins for the two divisions in the firm, Division A and Division B.

3.1.2. Part B of the Presented Learning Problem

Part B required problem solving at third-order and fourthorder levels. Learners were required to calculate the minimum transfer price that Division A would accept for selling to Division B if market forces influenced the transfer price. Understanding the meaning of transfer price requires understanding the meaning of several other concepts as follows.

(i) Know that outlay cost means variable costs incurred in production.

(ii) Know that opportunity cost means giving up a monetary benefit for choosing one course of action over another.

(iii) Be aware that excess capacity means the unused production quantity that remains after including production needs from internal and external customers.

(iv) Be mindful of the fact that transfer price of an item produced is an addition of outlay cost of an item and the opportunity cost of that item.

(v) Be aware of how to use these meanings and formulas simultaneously, to calculate outlay costs of Division A, and to calculate the opportunity cost of Division A.

3.1.3. Part C of the Presented Learning Problem

Part C required problem solving at the fifth-order level. Part C asked students to decide the overall impact of accepting or rejecting a special order received from a customer named Socceroos, on firm-wide profits. Answering Part C required students to know the following.

(i) Meaning of goal congruence, as a decision made by a division to make the most profit possible for the organisation, and not for the division itself.

(ii) Meanings of incremental revenue, incremental costs, and incremental profits. Incremental revenue is the extra revenue resulting from choosing one course of action against another. Incremental costs are the extra costs resulting from choosing one course of action against another. Incremental profit is extra profits resulting from choosing one course of action against another.

(iii) Know the connections of these meanings to calculate and decide whether the firm should accept or reject the special order.

a. First, calculate extra revenue earned by the firm.

b. Second, calculate extra costs incurred by division A for selling products to Division B because of the agreement between divisions.

c. Third, students needed to calculate extra costs that Division B incurs because of accepting the special order from a customer.

d. Fourth, students needed to calculate extra costs that Division A must incur in selling finished goods to Division B to meet the special order.

e. Fifth, they needed to calculate the opportunity costs that Division B must bear for accepting the special order.

f. Sixth, they needed to calculate extra profits earned by the firm.

Table 1 shows the transfer pricing problem provided to students in Phase 1 of this study. The problem had three parts (Part A, B, and C) to answer. Table 2 outlines the interview guide which explored students’ understandings of concepts.

Table 2: Model Solution

OTGHEU_2019_v6n1_9_t0002.png 이미지

Model Solution for Part A

Model Solution for Part B

Model Solution for Part C

3.2. Phase 2: Evaluating Answers to Ascertain Interventions and Conducting Interviews

3.2.1. Evaluating Answers

Students’ answers to the transfer pricing problem were evaluated against the model answer prepared during the research design stage (Table 2).

The findings from the previous semester assessments and student interviews led to the following improvements to decrease learner cognitive load.

(i) Provided a Transfer Pricing Workbook that students downloaded as prerequisite reading from the course learning site. The Workbook contained explanations of all the concepts required to learn transfer pricing in this course successfully. In contrast to the previous semester, the concepts about transfer pricing were covered in the prerequisite course as essential learning outcomes.

(ii) The Transfer Pricing Workbook provided students with blank pages where they were also asked to represent the problem using diagrams.

(iii) The lecturer highlighted the common misconceptions diagnosed in the previous semester evaluation.

(iv) The lecturer drew diagrams at each stage of problem solving to visually show the how the solution is researched.

(v) The lecturer gave students time to complete the examples before providing the solution. These activities provided students with opportunities for incidental learning.

3.2.2. Conducting Interviews and Documenting Feedback on Learning Difficulties

The interviews held with students revealed that students' wrong answers for Part A arose due to five reasons (see Appendix for interview excerpts with feedback from students). First, presenting information as numbers wherever possible, rather than as text, decreased the cognitive load. Second, students made mistakes because of conceptual misunderstandings. For example, they did not understand the difference between selling price (for Division A) and the market price. Third, students could not combine lower-order concepts with higher-order concepts and simultaneously use them. For example, in the Phase 1 findings for Part B, calculating contribution margin of Division A needed a student to interact with several items of information simultaneously. Fourth, interacting with these items of information cascaded over three levels, making learning to calculate contribution margin difficult. At the firstorder level, students needed to know the meaning of direct materials, direct labour, and variable overheads. At the second-order level, students had to know that these three types of cost make up variable production costs. At the third-order level, they should be aware that variable production costs become outlay costs for transfer pricing calculation. Students also should be aware the meanings of selling price, variable production costs, and non-variable production costs. At the second-order level, they should also understand how the existence of surplus production capacity can influence the opportunity costs. At the thirdorder level, they should be aware of the meaning of opportunity costs, and the information items that comprise these costs. At the fourth-order level, students should be aware that the outlay costs and opportunity costs of Division A production make up transfer pricing. When students reach the fifth-order level, they should be aware of goal congruence. They should explore whether the firm has a transfer pricing agreement to perform goal congruence, and accordingly calculate the contribution margin (Figure 1).

Fifth, the Phase 1 of the findings also showed that the information presented in the transfer pricing problem required students to split their attention between the text and footnotes. Splitting their attention in order to integrate information to understand the question also increased the cognitive load. The problem question had an asterisk (*), hat (^), and footnotes. These interruptions distracted students from reading the text and forced them to split their attention to find out what those pointers were asking them to read, so as to understand the material. For instance, an asterisk forced them to divide their attention underneath a table to realise that 50 percent of manufacturing overheads do not change, and 50 percent vary with the quantity of production.

3.3. Phase 3: Post-intervention

The same lecturer who conducted lectures in the first semester conducted lectures in the following semester. The lecture covered the theoretical aspects of transfer pricing, followed by practical examples using an overhead projector. The sequence of events mirrored the first semester (preintervention phase), with 25 students being invited after the mid-semester break to take part in the study in Phase 3 (post-intervention phase). Students’ GPA and average marks were not statistically significant from the Phase 1 cohort. The task required students to solve a semistructured transfer pricing problem that was conceptually identical to that performed by students in Phase 1. The equivalence was determined by showing the two transfer pricing problems to three academics who had taught transfer pricing, and obtaining unanimous agreement. The marking criteria for the solution to the problem were identical between pre-intervention and post-intervention, moderated by improvements to extraneous cognitive load to ease learner difficulties. The different but equivalent transfer pricing problem given to students in Phase 3 is shown in Table 3.

Table 3: Transfer Pricing Problem in Phase 3

OTGHEU_2019_v6n1_9_t0003.png 이미지

4. Results and Discussion

4.1. Results of Part A

Table 4 provides summary statistics of calculating selling price, contribution margin, and transfer price for Division A and Division B of the firm. The improvements due to interventions are significantly different from pre- to postintervention (X2(2) =385.53, p<0.05), and support H1. As additional tests, we measured the improvements at item level, and the intervention-improvements had a statistically significant effect at 5 percent significance level (exceeds the X2(1) critical value = 0.004).

Table 4: Correct answers by students in Part A before and after intervention (N = 25)​​​​​​​

OTGHEU_2019_v6n1_9_t0004.png 이미지

4.2. Results of Part B

Table 5 reports the proportion of correct answers obtained by solving Part B in Phase 1 and Phase 3. The improvements due to interventions are significantly different from pre- to post-intervention (X2(1) =14.67, p<0.05) and support H2. In Part B, the market forces decided the transfer price from Division A. This part also stated that Division A had extra packaging costs. These costs vary with production quantity and therefore are also variable costs. Below is an example of a common student error in Part B, where students wrongly calculated the transfer price. The transfer pricing rule yields a minimum acceptable transfer price of $85 to Division B. As additional tests, we measured the improvements at item level, and the interventionimprovements had a statistically significant effect at 5 percent significance level (exceeds the X2(1) critical value = 0.004).

Table 5: Correct answers by students in Part B (N = 25)​​​​​​​

OTGHEU_2019_v6n1_9_t0005.png 이미지

4.3. Results of Part C

Table 6 reports the proportion of correct answers obtained by solving Part C where students should use the transfer pricing rule to calculate the transfer price from Division A. Division B has an excess capacity to meet a special order at $200 per (sportswear) item. Part C assumes that excess production capacity exists in Division A, and Division B has requested a special-order production from Division A. The first step in this section was to recognise the meaning of goal congruence where divisional performances result in increasing profits of the firm. Forty-eight percent of students lacked such understanding. In answering Part C to calculate incremental profits to the firm, students made the following common errors. The improvements due to interventions are significantly different from pre- to post-intervention (X2(4) =226.76, p<0.05) and support H3. As additional tests, we measured the improvements at item level, and the intervention-improvements had a significant effect (exceeds the X2(1) critical value = 0.004).

Table 6: Correct answers by students in Part C (N = 25)​​​​​​​

OTGHEU_2019_v6n1_9_t0006.png 이미지

4.4. Reflections on Teaching Directors about Transfer Pricing

Most directors are novices to learning transfer pricing, and findings from this study using novice learners become applicable to teaching directors about transfer pricing. Consistent with Blaney et al. (2010), these findings led us to design a teaching format that isolated each information item about transfer pricing from lowest order to highest order, and to teach them sequentially. The interrelating of these information items then occurred progressively at higher order levels (Figure 1).

The post-intervention phase of the study attended to three areas: (i) conceptual difficulties were relieved by providing learners with, and guiding them with, a transfer pricing workbook, (ii) instructors understood the conceptual and procedural difficulties that learners have in learning transfer pricing, and learning was guided accordingly, and (iii) learners were encouraged to draw diagrams in order to visualise the text.

In adopting these interventions, the study replaced the problem-solving technique with the worked example technique (Chandler & Sweller, 1991). The problem-solving instructions presents learners with a description of the problem and a goal statement but does not provide adequate guidance about the procedures to solve the problem, leading to trial and error or means-solutions analysis strategies, although the learner may eventually find a solution. Worked examples provide learners with not only means-solutions analysis but also steps taken successively to solve the problem and achieve the solution state. Instructions that directs learners on studying worked examples reduces extraneous or ineffective cognitive load on the working memory which can enhance the learning through transfer of learning from working memory to long term memory (Spanjers, van Gog, & van Merrienboer, 2012).

As found in Phase 3, the teaching instruction should also add diagrams to show directors how to interrelate information items in transfer pricing. As found in this study, there are seven sequential instructional visual steps to follow in transfer pricing: 1. Drawing two boxes, one to represent Division A and the other Division B. 2. Drawing an arrow labelled “transfer price” from Division A to Division B, indicating that the transfer price between the divisions was yet to be determined. 3. Drawing a box around Division A and Division B together, to represent their relationship as divisions within the one company, recognising a need for goal congruence. 4. Drawing two boxes external to the company box, labelling the first box “External Market for Division A” and the other “External Market for Division B.” 5. Drawing an arrow from Division A out to the external market for Division A and labelling the output with the market selling price. 6. Drawing an arrow from Division B out to the external market for Division B and labelling the output with the market selling price. 7. Drawing an arrow from the external market for Division A into Division B, labelled with the input price if Division B was forced to buy its input from the open market rather than by internal transfer from Division A.

4.5. Limitations and Future Research

In interpreting findings, there are three limitations to be acknowledged. First, it is neither claimed here that cognitive load theory is the only way to explain these results, nor contended that it explains all reasons behind the learning difficulties learners experienced. For instance, cognitive load theory does not consider the influences of psychological reasons such as student beliefs, expectations, and goals (Bannert, 2002). Willingness to invest time and effort in learning the task, and learners' views of achieving personal goals are also not considered here (Thrash & Elliott, 2001). Second, the improved learning outcomes are due to easing out conceptual difficulties, and students transforming the problem from text into visual. This study did not measure the contribution of each improvement-intervention measure separately and this is a future research proposal. However, the learning difficulties identified in this study can help in improving teaching design to help learners learn complicated topics such as transfer pricing. These topics require learning lower-level and higher-level ideas and interrelating them, to decrease the cognitive load in working memory. Third, the study randomly selected 25 students from among those who expressed interest in taking part in the study, but omitting those who did not express interest may have contributed to a selection bias. These limitations can be used for future research to deepen the understanding about learning a vital topic such as transfer pricing.

References

  1. Bannert, M. (2002). Managing cognitive load, recent trends in cognitive load theory. Learning and Instruction, 12, 139-146. https://doi.org/10.1016/S0959-4752(01)00021-4
  2. Blayney, P., Kalyuga, S., & Sweller, J. (2010). Interactions between the isolated-interactive elements effect and levels of learner expertise: experimental evidence from an accountancy class. Instructional Science, 38(3), 277-287 https://doi.org/10.1007/s11251-009-9105-x
  3. Blayney, P., Kalyuga, S., & Sweller, J. (2016). The impact of complexity on the expertise reversal effect: experimental evidence from testing accounting students. Educational Psychology, 36, 1868-1885. https://doi.org/10.1080/01443410.2015.1051949
  4. CAANZ (Chartered Accountants in Australia and New Zealand). (2016, June 24). Financial literacy of directors survey results. Retrieved from http://www.charteredaccountants.com.au/Industry-Topics/Reporting/News-and-guidance-onregulatory-matters/News-and-updates/Financialliteracy-of-directors-survey-results.
  5. Chandler, P., & Sweller, J. (1991). Cognitive load theory and the format of instruction. Cognition and Instruction, 8(4), 293-332. https://doi.org/10.1207/s1532690xci0804_2
  6. Kahneman, D. (1973). Attention and effort. Englewood Cliffs, NJ: Prentice-Hall.
  7. Langfield-Smith, K., Thorne, H., & Hilton, R. W. (2006). Management accounting: Information for managing and creating value (4th ed.). Sydney, Australia: McGraw-Hill.
  8. Lee, H., Plass, J. J., & Homer, B. D. (2006). Optimizing cognitive load for learning from computer science simulations. Journal of Educational Psychology, 98, 902-913. https://doi.org/10.1037/0022-0663.98.4.902
  9. Miller, G. A. (1956). The magical number seven, plus or minus two: Some limits on capacity for processing information. Psychological Review, 63, 81-97. https://doi.org/10.1037/h0043158
  10. Moreno, R., & Park, B. (2010). Cognitive load theory: Historical development and relation to other theories (chapter one). In J. L. Plass, R. Moreno, & R. Brunken (Eds.), Cognitive load theory. New York, NY: Cambridge University Press.
  11. Pollock, E., Chandler, P., & Sweller, J. (2002). Assimilating complex information. Learning and Instruction, 12, 61-86. https://doi.org/10.1016/S0959-4752(01)00016-0
  12. Power, B. (2012.) Transfer pricing: Are borders meaningless? Retrieved from http://www.charteredaccountants.com.au/News-Media/Charter/Charter-articles/Business-management/2012-09-Are-Borders-Meaningless.aspx.
  13. Schneider, W., & Schiffrin, R. M. (1977). Controlled and automatic human information processing: 1. Detection, search, and attention. Psychological Review, 84, 1-66. https://doi.org/10.1037/0033-295X.84.1.1
  14. Sithole, S. T. M., Chandler, P., Abeysekera, I., & Paas, F. (2017). Benefits of guided self-management of attention on learning accounting. Journal of Educational Psychology, 109(2), 220-232. DOI:10.1037/edu0000127.
  15. Spanjers, I. A. E., van Gog, T., & van Merrienboer, J. J. G. (2012). Segmentation of worked examples: Effects on cognitive load and learning. Applied Cognitive Psychology, 26, 352-358. https://doi.org/10.1002/acp.1832
  16. Sweller, J. (1994). Cognitive load theory, learning difficulty, and instructional design. Learning and Instruction, 4, 295-312. https://doi.org/10.1016/0959-4752(94)90003-5
  17. Sweller, J., & Chandler, P. (1994). Why some material is difficult to learn. Cognition and Instruction, 12(3), 185-233. https://doi.org/10.1207/s1532690xci1203_1
  18. Sweller, J., Ayers, P., & Kalyuga, S. (2011). Cognitive load theory. New York, NY: Springer.
  19. Thrash, T., & Elliott, A. (2001). Delimiting and integrating achievement motive and goal constructs. In A. Efklides, J. Kuhl, & R. Sorrentino (Eds.), Trends and prospects in motivation research (pp.3-21). Boston, MA: Kluwer.
  20. van Merrienboer, J., Kirschner, P. A., & Kester, L. (2003). Taking the load off a learner's mind: Instructional design for complex learning. Educational Psychology, 38, 5-13. https://doi.org/10.1207/S15326985EP3801_2

Cited by

  1. Effectiveness of Learning Performances According to Financial Motivation of University Students vol.9, pp.3, 2019, https://doi.org/10.13106/jbees.2019.vol9.no3.27
  2. The Effect of Changes of Learning Systems on Learning Outcomes in COVID-19 Pandemic Conditions vol.8, pp.3, 2021, https://doi.org/10.13106/jafeb.2021.vol8.no3.0695
  3. The Effect of Ownership Structure on Transfer Pricing Decisions: Evidence from Foreign Direct Investments in Vietnam vol.8, pp.12, 2019, https://doi.org/10.13106/jafeb.2021.vol8.no12.0183