DOI QR코드

DOI QR Code

유아의 다중과제 수행과 심리적 불응기: PRP 패러다임 과제를 중심으로

Multiple Task Performance and Psychological Refractory Period in Children: Focusing on PRP Paradigm Tasks

  • 김보경 (서울대학교 생활과학대학 아동가족학과) ;
  • 이순형 (서울대학교 생활과학대학 아동가족학과)
  • Kim, Bokyung (Department of Child Development and Family Studies, College of Human Ecology, Seoul National University) ;
  • Yi, Soon Hyung (Department of Child Development and Family Studies, College of Human Ecology, Seoul National University)
  • 투고 : 2017.05.07
  • 심사 : 2017.06.12
  • 발행 : 2017.06.30

초록

Objective: This study aimed to identify children's cognitive processing and performance characteristics while multiple task performance. It confirmed whether their multiple task performance and psychological refractory period (PRP) varied by task condition (stimulus onset asynchrony [SOA] and task difficulty) and stimulus modality. Methods: Seventy 5-year-olds were recruited. Multi-task tools were developed using the E-prime software. The children were required to respond to two stimuli (visual or auditory) presented with microscopic time difference and their response times (RTs) were recorded. Results: As the SOA increased, the RTs in the first task increased, while the RTs in the second task and PRP decreased. The RTs of the first and second tasks, and the PRP for difficult tasks, were significantly longer than those for easy tasks were. Additionally, there was an interaction effect between the SOA and task difficulty. Although there was no main effect of stimulus modality, task difficulty moderated the modality effect. In the high difficulty condition, the RTs of the first and second tasks and PRP for the visual-visual task were significantly longer than those for auditory-auditory task were. Conclusion: These results inform theoretical discussions on children's multi-task mechanism, and the loss of multiple task performance. Additionally, they provide practical implications and information on the composition of multi-tasks suitable for children in educational environments.

키워드

참고문헌

  1. Arnell, K. M., & Duncan, J. (2002). Separate and shared sources of dual-task cost in stimulus identification and response selection. Cognitive Psychology, 44(2), 105-147. doi:10.1006/cogp.2001.0762
  2. Badwe, N., Patil, K. B., Yelam, S. B., Vikhe, B. B., & Vatve, M. S. (2012). A comparative study of hand reaction time to visual stimuli in students of 1st MBBS of a rural medical college. Pravara Medical Review, 4(1), 4-6.
  3. Becker, D. E., Miao, A., Duncan, R., & McClelland, M. M. (2014). Behavioral self-regulation and executive function both predict visuomotor skills and early academic achievement. Early Childhood Research Quarterly, 29(4), 411-424. doi:10.1016/j.ecresq.2014.04.014
  4. Besner, D., Reynolds, M., & O’Malley, S. (2009). When underadditivity of factor effects in the Psychological Refractory Period paradigm implies a bottleneck: Evidence from psycholinguistics. The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 62(11), 2222-2234. doi:10.1080/17470210902 747187
  5. Bherer, L., Kramer, A. F., Peterson, M. S., Colcombe, S., Erickson, K., & Becic, E. (2005). Training effects on dual-task performance: Are there age-related differences in plasticity of attentional control? Psychology and Aging, 20(4), 695- 709. doi:10.1037/0882-7974.20.4.695
  6. Birch, L. L. (1976). Age trends in children's time-sharing performance. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 22(2), 331-345. doi:10.1016/0022-0965(76)90013-8
  7. Bjorklund, D. F., & Hamishfeger, K. K. (1987). Developmental differences in the mental effort requirements for the use of an organizational strategy in free recall. Journal of Experimental Child Ps ycholog y, 44(1), 109-125. doi:10.1016/0022- 0965(87)90025-7
  8. Bogartz, R. S. (1976). On the meaning of statistical interactions. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 22(1), 178-183. doi:10.1016/0022-0965(76)90099-0
  9. Brisson, B., & Jolicoeur, P. (2007). Cross-modal multitasking processing deficits prior to the central bottleneck revealed by event-related potentials. Neuropsychologia, 45(13), 3038- 3053. doi:10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2007.05.022
  10. Bruce, V., Green, P. R., & Georgeson, M. A. (1996). Visual perception: Physiology, psychology, and ecology (3rd ed.). East Sussex, UK: Psychology Press.
  11. Buhner, M., König, C. J., Pick, M., & Krumm, S. (2006). Working memory dimensions as differential predictors of the speed and error aspect of multitasking performance. Human Performance, 19(3), 253-275. doi:10.1207/ s15327043hup1903_4
  12. Casey, B. J., Galvan, A., & Hare, T. (2005). Changes in cerebral functional organization during cognitive development. Current Opinion in Neurobiology, 15(2), 239-244. doi:10.1016/j.conb.2005.03.012
  13. Colom, R., Martínez-Molina, A., Shih, P. C., & Santacreu, J. (2010). Intelligence, working memory, and multitasking performance. Intelligence, 38(6), 543-551. doi:10.1016/ j.intell.2010.08.002
  14. Courage, M. L., Bakhtiar, A., Fitzpatrick, C., Kenny, S., & Brandeau, K. (2015). Growing up multitasking: The costs and benefits for cognitive development. Developmental Review, 35, 5-41. doi:10.1016/j.dr.2014.12.002
  15. Fischer, R., & Hommel, B. (2012). Deep thinking increases taskset shielding and reduces shifting flexibility in dual-task performance. Cognition, 123(2), 303-307. doi:10.1016/ j.cognition.2011.11.015
  16. Garon, N., Bryson, S. E., & Smith, I. M. (2008). Executive function in preschoolers: A review using an integrative framework. Psychological Bulletin, 134(1), 31-60. doi:10.1037/0033-2909.134.1.31
  17. Ghirardelli, T. G., & Scharine, A. A. (2009). Auditory-visual interactions. In C. E. Rash, M. B. Russo, T. Letowski, & E. Schmeisser (Eds.), Helmet-mounted displays: Sensation, perception and cognition issues (pp. 599-618). Ft. Rucker, AL: US Army Aeromedical Research Lab.
  18. Grondin, S., Roussel, M. E., Gamache, P. L., Roy, M., & Ouellet, B. (2005). The structure of sensory events and the accuracy of time judgments. Perception, 34(1), 45-58. doi:10.1068/ p5369
  19. Halford, G. S., Maybery, M. T., & Bain, J. D. (1986). Capacity limitations in children’s reasoning: A dual-task approach. Child Development, 57(3), 616-627. doi:10.2307/1130340
  20. Hibberd, D. L., Jamson, S. L., & Carsten, O. M. J. (2010). Managing in-vehicle distractions: Evidence from the Psychological Refractory Period paradigm. Proceedings of the 2nd International Conference on Automotive User Interfaces and Interactive Vehicular Applications, 4-11. doi:10.1145/1969773.1969775
  21. Hibberd, D. L., Jamson, S. L., & Carsten, O. M. J. (2013). Mitigating the effects of in-vehicle distractions through use of the Psychological Refractory Period paradigm. Accident Analysis and Prevention, 50, 1096-1103. doi:10.1016/ j.aap.2012.08.016
  22. Holtzer, R., Stern, Y., & Rakitin, B. C. (2005). Predicting agerelated dual-task effects with individual differences on neuropsychological tests. Neuropsychology, 19(1), 18-27. doi:10.1037/0894-4105.19.1.18
  23. Irwin-Chase, H., & Burns, B. (2000). Developmental changes in children’s abilities to share and allocate attention in a dual task. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 77(1), 61-85. doi:10.1006/jecp.1999.2557
  24. Janczyk, M., Pfister, R., Hommel, B., & Kunde, W. (2014). Who is talking in backward crosstalk? Disentangling responsefrom goal-conflict in dual-task performance. Cognition, 132(1), 30-43. doi:10.1016/j.cognition.2014.03.001
  25. Kee, D. W., & Davies, L. (1988). Mental effort and elaboration: A development analysis. Contemporary Educational Psychology, 13(3), 221-228. doi:10.1016/0361-476X(88)90022-7
  26. Krumbholz, K., Patterson, R. D., Nobbe, A., & Fastl, H. (2003). Microsecond temporal resolution in monaural hearing without spectral cues? Journal of the Acoustical Society of America, 113(5), 2790-2800. doi:10.1121/1.1547438
  27. Lague-Beauvais, M., Gagnon, C., Castonguay, N., & Bherer, L. (2013). Individual differences effects on the psychological refractory period. SpringerPlus, 2(1), 368. doi:10.1186/2193-1801-2-368
  28. Leonard, J. A. (1959). Tactual choice reactions: I. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 11(2), 76-83. doi:10.1080/17470215908416294
  29. Leshowitz, B. (1971). Measurement of the two-click threshold. Journal of the Acoustical Society of America, 49(2B), 462- 466. doi:10.1121/1.1912374
  30. Lien, M. C., & Proctor, R. W. (2002). Stimulus-response compatibility and psychological refractory period effects: Implications for response selection. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 9(2), 212-238. doi:10.3758/BF03196277
  31. Logan, G. D., & Gordon, R. D. (2001). Executive control of visual attention in dual-task situations. Psychological Review, 108(2), 393-434. doi:10.1037/0033-295X.108.2.393
  32. Luc iana, M. , & Ne l son, C. (2002) . Assessment of neuropsychological function through use of the Cambridge Neuropsychological Testing Automated Battery: Performance in 4- to 12-year-old children. Developmental Neuropsychology, 22(3), 595-624. doi:10.1207/S15326942DN2203_3
  33. McCann, R. S., & Johnston, J. C. (1992). Locus of the singlechannel bottleneck in dual-task interference. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 18(2), 471-484. doi:10.1037/0096-1523.18.2.471
  34. Mioni, G., Grondin, S., Forgione, M., Fracasso, V., Mapelli, D., & Stablum, F. (2016). The role of primary auditory and visual cortices in temporal processing: A tDCS approach. Behavioural Brain Research, 313, 151-157. doi:10.1016/ j.bbr.2016.07.019
  35. Pashler, H. (1994). Dual-task Interference in simple tasks: Data and theory. Psychological Bulletin, 116(2), 220-244. doi:10.1037/0033-2909.116.2.220
  36. Rammsayer, T. H. (2014). The effects of type of interval, sensory modality, base duration, and psychophysical task on the discrimination of brief time intervals. Attention, Perception, & Psychophysics, 76(4), 1185-1196. doi:10.3758/s13414- 014-0655-x
  37. Van Selst, M., & Jolicoeur, P. (1994). Can mental rotation occur before the dual-task bottleneck? Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 20(4), 905-921. doi:10.1037/0096-1523.20.4.905
  38. Welford, A. T. (1952). The "psychological refractory period" and the timing of high speed performance: A review and theory. British Journal of Psychology, 43(1), 2-19. doi:10.1111/ j.2044-8295.1952.tb00322.x
  39. Williams, J., Rickert, V., Hogan, J., Zolten, A. J., Satz, P., D'Elia, L. F., . . . Light, R. (1995). Children's color trails. Archives of Clinical Neuropsychology, 10(3), 211-223. doi:10.1093/ arclin/10.3.211
  40. Zelazo, P. D., & Carlson, S. M. (2012). Hot and cool executive function in childhood and adolescence: Development and plasticity. Child Development Perspectives, 6(4), 354-360. doi:10.1111/j.1750-8606.2012.00246.x
  41. Chung, C.-H., & Kim, A.-S. (1991). The effects of inter-stimulus intervals and number of alternatives on psychological refractory period. Korean Journal of Sport Psychology, 2(1), 3-12.
  42. Hong, S. H. (2005). Difference in information processing capacity by age and sex of elementary and middle school students (Master's thesis). Retrieved from http://www.riss.kr/ link?id=T10699483
  43. Koo, H. J., & Shin, M. S. (2008). A standardization study of children’s color trails test (CCTT). Journal of the Korean Academy of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry, 19(1), 28-37.
  44. Park, G. (2002). Effects of the inter-stimulus intervals and the athletic experience on psychological refractory period (Master's thesis). Retrieved from http://www.riss.kr/link?id=T9957822
  45. Park, G., & Lee, K.-H. (2003). Effects of the inter-stimulus intervals and the athletic experience on psychological refractory period. Korean Journal of Sports Science, 12(2), 245-254. Retrieved from http://www.dbpia.co.kr/Article/ NODE00828370
  46. Song, E.-S. (1995). Effects of the length of inter-stimulus intervals, number of stimulus-response alternatives, sport experience and sex on the psychological refractory period. Korean Journal of Sport Psychology, 6(2), 1-16.