DOI QR코드

DOI QR Code

Affective Priming Effect on Cognitive Processes Reflected by Event-related Potentials

ERP로 확인되는 인지정보 처리에 대한 정서 점화효과

  • 김충명 (광주대학교 언어.심리치료학부)
  • Received : 2015.12.30
  • Accepted : 2016.02.22
  • Published : 2016.05.28

Abstract

This study was conducted to investigate whether Stroop-related cognitive task will be affected according to the preceding affective valence factored by matchedness in response time(RT) and whether facial recognition will be indexed by specific event-related potentials(ERPs) signature in normal person as in patients suffering from affective disorder. ERPs primed by subliminal(30ms) facial stimuli were recorded when presented with four pairs of affect(positive or negative) and cognitive task(matched or mismatched) to get ERP effects(N2 and P300) in terms of its amplitude and peak latency variations. Behavioral response analysis based on RTs confirmed that subliminal affective stimuli primed the target processing in all affective condition except for the neutral stimulus. Additional results for the ERPs performed in the negative affect with mismatched condition reached significance of emotional-face specificity named N2 showing more amplitude and delayed peak latency compared to the positive counterpart. Furthermore the condition shows more positive amplitude and earlier peak latency of P300 effect denoting cognitive closure than the corresponding positive affect condition. These results are suggested to reflect that negative affect stimulus in subliminal level is automatically inhibited such that this effect had influence on accelerating detection of the affect and facilitating response allowing adequate reallocation of attentional resources. The functional and cognitive significance with these findings was implied in terms of subliminal effect and affect-related recognition modulating the cognitive tasks.

본 연구는 스트룹(Stroop)과제를 이용하여 정서가(affective valence)로 구성된 역하의 정서유발 점화자극이 인지과제 수행에 영향을 미치는지와 일반인에서도 정서장애 환자와 같은 지표가 나타나는지를 반응시간과 ERP(사건관련전위) 패턴으로 확인해 보고자 하였다. 측정된 데이터는 정서가(긍정 및 부정)와 일치여부로 수행되는 인지과제의 조합으로 제시되어 수집되었고, 전위변화의 특성인 진폭과 정점 잠복기를 기준으로 그 효과가 검증되었다. 먼저 반응시간 행동분석 결과, 중립 정서자극을 제외한 역하의 정서자극에 의해 목표과제의 반응을 촉진하는 결과를 확인할 수 있었다. 아울러 부가적으로 수집된 사건관련전위를 분석한 결과, 불일치 요인이 결합된 부정적 정서정보가 긍정적 정서정보에 비해 더 높은 음전위 및 지연된 잠복기를 보임으로써 표정과 관련된 정서유발-특정적인 N2효과를 확인하였다. 그리고 동일한 조건에서 긍정조건에 비해 유의한 차이의 양전위와 함께 더 짧은 잠복기를 보이는, 인지적 판단과정의 차이를 시사하는 P300의 효과도 관찰하였다. 이는 역하의 부정적 정서정보가 인지처리과정에서 자동적으로 억제되는 경향과 함께 해당 정서의 탐지를 가속시키는 한편, 주의자원의 적절한 재분배를 가능케 하여 목표자극의 반응을 촉진시킨 것으로 해석할 수 있으며, 기능적이고 인지적인 반응의 차이에서 역하효과를 비롯하여 과제 수행 정도에 영향을 끼치는 정서관련 재인과정의 중요성을 시사한다 할 수 있다.

Keywords

References

  1. J. M. G. Williams, F. N. Watts, C. MacLeod, and A. Mathews, Cognitive psychology and emotional disorders (2nd ed.), Chichester: UK: Wiley., 1997.
  2. A. Mathews and C. MacLeod, "Selective processing of threat cues in anxiety states," Behaviour Research & Therapy, Vol.23, pp.563-569, 1985. https://doi.org/10.1016/0005-7967(85)90104-4
  3. K. Mogg, A. Mathews, and J. Weinman, "Selecting processing of threat cues in anxiety states: A replication," Behavior Research and Therapy, Vol.27, pp.317-323, 1989. https://doi.org/10.1016/0005-7967(89)90001-6
  4. B. P. Bradley, K. Mogg, and S. Lee, "Attentional biases for negative information in induced and naturally occurring dysphoria," Behavior Research and Therapy, Vol.35, pp.911-927, 1997. https://doi.org/10.1016/S0005-7967(97)00053-3
  5. B. P. Bradley, K. Mogg, J. White, C. Groom, and J. de Bono, "Attentional bias for emotional faces in generalised anxiety disorder," British Journal of Clinical Psychology, Vol.38, pp.267-278, 1999. https://doi.org/10.1348/014466599162845
  6. Y. Bar-Haim, D. Lamy, L. Pergamin, M. Bakersmans-Kranenburg, and M. H. van Ijzendoorn, "Threat-related attentional bias in anxious and nonanxious individuals: A meta-analytic study," Psychological Bulletin, Vol.133, No.1, pp.1-24, 2007. https://doi.org/10.1037/0033-2909.133.1.1
  7. D. Hermans, D. Vansteenwegen, and P. Eelen, "Eye Movement Registration as a Continuous Index of Attention Deployment: Date from a Group of Spider Anxious Students," Cognition and Emotion, Vol.13, No.4, pp.419-434, 1999. https://doi.org/10.1080/026999399379249
  8. M. W. Eysenck, Anxiety: the cognitive perceptive, Hove, UK: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates Ltd. 1992.
  9. E. Fox, R. Russo, R. Bowles, and K. Dutton, "Do threatening stimuli dray or hold visual attention in subclinical anxiety?," Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, Vol.130, pp.681-700, 2001. https://doi.org/10.1037/0096-3445.130.4.681
  10. C. MacLeod, A. Mathews, and P. Tata, "Attentional bias in emotional disorders," Journal of Abnormal Psychology, Vol.95, pp.15-20, 1986. https://doi.org/10.1037/0021-843X.95.1.15
  11. B. P. Bradley, K. Mogg, and N. H. Millar, "Covert and overt orienting of attention to emotional faces in anxiety," Cognition and Emotion, Vol.14, pp.789-808, 2000. https://doi.org/10.1080/02699930050156636
  12. J. Yiend and A. Mathews, "Anxiety and attention to threatening pictures," Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology:Human Experimental Psychology, Vol.54, pp.665-681, 2001. https://doi.org/10.1080/713755991
  13. J. M. G. Williams, A. Mathews, and C. MacLeod, "The Emotional Stroop Task and Psychopathology," Psychological Bulletin, Vol.120, pp.3-24, 1996. https://doi.org/10.1037/0033-2909.120.1.3
  14. A. Mathews and C. MacLeod, "Induced processing biases have causal effects on anxiety," Cognition and Emotion, Vol.16, pp.331-354, 2002. https://doi.org/10.1080/02699930143000518
  15. M. G. Choi, "Attentional Bias Effect across the Emotional Valence in Normal Population," Korean Journal of Experimental Psychology, Vol.17, No.2, pp.111-130, 2005.
  16. F. Di Russo, F. Taddei, T. Aprile, and D. Spinelli, "Neural correlates of fast stimulus discrimination and response selection in top-level fencers," Neuroscience Letters, Vol.408, pp.113-118, 2006. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neulet.2006.08.085
  17. L. Carretie, J. A. Hinojosa, M. Martin-Loeches, F. Mercado, and M. Tapia, "Automatic attention to emotional stimuli: neural correlates," Human Brain Mapping, Vol.22, pp.290-299, 2004. https://doi.org/10.1002/hbm.20037
  18. A. Azizian and J. Polich, "Evidence for attentional gradient in the serial position memory curve from ERPs," Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, Vol.19, No.12, pp.2071-2081, 2007. https://doi.org/10.1162/jocn.2007.19.12.2071
  19. D. Sabatinelli, P. J. Lang, A. Keil, and M. M. Bradley, "Emotional perception: Correlation of functional MRI and event-related potentials," Cerebral Cortex, Vol.17, pp.1085-1091, 2007.
  20. J. Polich, "Updating P300: an integrative theory of P3a and P3b," Clinical Neurophysiology, Vol.118, pp.2128-2148, 2007. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.clinph.2007.04.019
  21. D. Lundqvist, F. Esteves, and A. Ohman, "The face of wrath: Critical features for conveying facial threat," Cognition and Emotion, Vol.13, pp.691-711, 1999. https://doi.org/10.1080/026999399379041
  22. L. Carretie, J. Iglesias, and T. Garcia, "A Study on the Emotional Processing of Visual Stimuli through Event-Related Potentials," Brain and Cognition, Vol.34, No.2, pp.207-217, 1997. https://doi.org/10.1006/brcg.1997.0895
  23. S. Bentin, T.Allison, A. Puce, E. Perez, and G. McCarthy, "Electrophysiological studies of face perception in humans," Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, Vol.8, No.6, pp.551-565, 1995. https://doi.org/10.1162/jocn.1996.8.6.551
  24. X. Q. Mai, J. Luo, J. H. Wu, and Y. J. Luo, "'Aha!' effects in guessing riddle task: An ERP study," Human Brain Mapping, Vol.22, pp.261-270, 2004. https://doi.org/10.1002/hbm.20030
  25. J. Qiu, H. Li, Y. J. Luo, A. T. Chen, and Q. L. Zhang, "Brain mechanism of cognitive conflict in a guessing Chinese logogriph task," NeuroReport, Vol.17, pp.679-682, 2006. https://doi.org/10.1097/00001756-200604240-00025