Acknowledgement
Supported by : 한국연구재단
References
- P. W. Glimcher and A. Rustichini, "Neuroeconomics: the consilience of brain and decision," Science, Vol. 306, No. 5695, pp. 447-452, 2004. https://doi.org/10.1126/science.1102566
- A. G. Sanfey, "Social decision-making: insights from game theory and neuroscience," Science, Vol. 318, No. 5850, pp. 598-602, 2007. https://doi.org/10.1126/science.1142996
- R. F. Baumeister and B. J. Bushman, Social Psychology and Human Nature. Belmont: Thomson Wadsworth, 2008.
- J. F. Kihlstrom, J. S. Beer, and S. B. Klein, "Self and identity as memory," in Handbook of Self and Identity, L. M. R. and J. P. Tangney, Eds., ed New York: Guilford Press, 2003, pp. 68-90.
- D. T. Gilbert and T. D. Wilson, "Prospection: experiencing the future," Science, Vol. 317, No. 5843, pp. 1351-1354, 2007. https://doi.org/10.1126/science.1144161
- R. H. Thaler and H. M. Shefrin, "An economic theory of self-control," J. Polit. Econ., Vol. 89, pp. 392-406, 1981. https://doi.org/10.1086/260971
- Y. Trope and N. Liberman, "Temporal construal," Psychol. Rev., Vol. 110, No. 3, pp. 403-421, 2003. https://doi.org/10.1037/0033-295X.110.3.403
- K. Lewin, Field theory in social science. New York: Harper, 1951.
- E. Pronin, C. Y. Olivola, and K. A. Kennedy, "Doing unto future selves as you would do unto others: psychological distance and decision making," Pers. Soc. Psychol. Bull., Vol. 34, No. 2, pp. 224-236, 2008.
- G. S. Berns, D. Laibson, and G. Loewenstein, "Intertemporal choice-toward an integrative framework," Trends. Cogn. Sci., Vol. 11, No. 11, pp. 482-488, 2007. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.tics.2007.08.011
- S. Frederick, G. Loewenstein, and T. O'Donoghue, "Time discounting and time preference: A critical review," J. Econ. Lit., Vol. 40, pp. 351-401, 2002. https://doi.org/10.1257/002205102320161311
- D. Prelec and G. Loewenstein, "Decision making over time and under uncetainty: a common approach," Manage. Sci., Vol. 37, No. 7, pp. 770-786, 1991. https://doi.org/10.1287/mnsc.37.7.770
- B. Jones and H. Rachlin, "Social discounting," Psychol. Sci., Vol. 17, No. 4, pp. 283-286, 2006. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-9280.2006.01699.x
- R. L. Buckner and D. C. Carroll, "Self-projection and the brain," Trends. Cogn. Sci., Vol. 11, No. 2, pp. 49- 57, 2007. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.tics.2006.11.004
- S. B. Klein, J. Loftus, and J. F. Kihlstrom, "Memory and temporal experience: The effects of episodic memory loss on an amnesic patient's ability to remember the past and imagine the future," Soc. Cogn., Vol. 20, No. 353-379, 2002. https://doi.org/10.1521/soco.20.5.353.21125
- D. Hassabis, D. Kumaran, S. D. Vann, and E. A. Maguire, "Patients with hippocampal amnesia cannot imagine new experiences," Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U. S. A., Vol. 104, No. 5, pp. 1726-1731, 2007. https://doi.org/10.1073/pnas.0610561104
- D. R. Addis, A. T. Wong, and D. L. Schacter, "Agerelated changes in the episodic simulation of future events," Psychol. Sci., Vol. 19, No. 1, pp. 33-41, 2008. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-9280.2008.02043.x
- K. B. Lyle, S. M. Bloise, and M. K. Johnson, "Agerelated binding deficits and the content of false memories," Psychol. Aging, Vol. 21, No. 1, pp. 86-95, 2006. https://doi.org/10.1037/0882-7974.21.1.86
- D. R. Addis, A. T. Wong, and D. L. Schacter, "Remembering the past and imagining the future: common and distinct neural substrates during event construction and elaboration," Neuropsychologia, Vol. 45, No. 7, pp. 1363-1377, 2007. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2006.10.016
- J. Okuda, T. Fujii, H. Ohtake, T. Tsukiura, K. Tanji, K. Suzuki, R. Kawashima, H. Fukuda, M. Itoh, and A. Yamadori, "Thinking of the future and past: the roles of the frontal pole and the medial temporal lobes," Neuroimage, Vol. 19, No. 4, pp. 1369-1380, 2003. https://doi.org/10.1016/S1053-8119(03)00179-4
- K. K. Szpunar, J. M. Watson, and K. B. McDermott, "Neural substrates of envisioning the future," Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U. S. A., Vol. 104, No. 2, pp. 642-647, 2007. https://doi.org/10.1073/pnas.0610082104
- J. L. Vincent, A. Z. Snyder, M. D. Fox, B. J. Shannon, J. R. Andrews, M. E. Raichle, and R. L. Buckner, "Coherent spontaneous activity identifies a hippocampalparietal memory network," J. Neurophysiol., Vol. 96, No. 6, pp. 3517-3531, 2006. https://doi.org/10.1152/jn.00048.2006
- D. L. Schacter, D. R. Addis, and R. L. Buckner, "Remembering the past to imagine the future: the prospective brain," Nat. Rev. Neurosci., Vol. 8, No. 9, pp. 657-661, 2007.
- T. D. Wilson and D. T. Gilbert, "Affective forecasting," in Advances in experimental social psychology. Vol. 35, M. Zanna, Ed., ed New York: Elsevier, 2003, pp. 345-411.
- W. Mischel, Y. Shoda, and M. I. Rodriguez, "Delay of gratification in children," Science, Vol. 244, No. 4907, pp. 933-938, 1989. https://doi.org/10.1126/science.2658056
- P. R. Montague and G. S. Berns, "Neural economics and the biological substrates of valuation," Neuron, Vol. 36, No. 2, pp. 265-284, 2002. https://doi.org/10.1016/S0896-6273(02)00974-1
- C. C. Luhmann, M. M. Chun, D. J. Yi, D. Lee, and X. J. Wang, "Neural dissociation of delay and uncertainty in intertemporal choice," J. Neurosci., Vol. 28, No. 53, pp. 14459-14466, 2008. https://doi.org/10.1523/JNEUROSCI.5058-08.2008
- G. G. Gallup, "Chimpanzees: self-recognition," Science, Vol. 167, pp. 86-87, 1970. https://doi.org/10.1126/science.167.3914.86
- D. J. Povinelli and T. M. Preuss, "Theory of Mind -Evolutionary History of a Cognitive Specialization," Trends Neurosci., Vol. 18, No. 9, pp. 418-424, 1995. https://doi.org/10.1016/0166-2236(95)93939-U
- D. Reiss and L. Marino, "Mirror self-recognition in the bottlenose dolphin: a case of cognitive convergence," Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U. S. A., Vol. 98, No. 10, pp. 5937-5942, 2001. https://doi.org/10.1073/pnas.101086398
- J. M. Plotnik, F. B. de Waal, and D. Reiss, "Self-recognition in an Asian elephant," Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U. S. A., Vol. 103, No. 45, pp. 17053-17057, 2006. https://doi.org/10.1073/pnas.0608062103
- H. Wimmer and J. Perner, "Beliefs about beliefs: representation and constraining function of wrong beliefs in young children's understanding of deception," Cognition, Vol. 13, No. 1, pp. 103-128, 1983. https://doi.org/10.1016/0010-0277(83)90004-5
- A. M. Leslie, "Pretense and representation in infancy: the origins of 'theory of mind'," Psychol. Rev., Vol. 1987, No. 94, pp. 412-426, 1987.
- D. S. Lindsay, M. K. Johnson, and P. Kwon, "Developmental changes in memory source monitoring," J. Exp. Child Psychol., Vol. 52, No. 3, pp. 297-318, 1991. https://doi.org/10.1016/0022-0965(91)90065-Z
- R. Saxe and N. Kanwisher, "People thinking about thinking people. The role of the temporo-parietal junction in 'theory of mind'," Neuroimage, Vol. 19, No. 4, pp. 1835-1842, 2003. https://doi.org/10.1016/S1053-8119(03)00230-1
- H. L. Gallagher, F. Happe, N. Brunswick, P. C. Fletcher, U. Frith, and C. D. Frith, "Reading the mind in cartoons and stories: an fMRI study of 'theory of mind' in verbal and nonverbal tasks," Neuropsychologia, Vol. 38, No. 1, pp. 11-21, 2000. https://doi.org/10.1016/S0028-3932(99)00053-6
- D. A. Gusnard, M. E. Raichle, and M. E. Raichle, "Searching for a baseline: functional imaging and the resting human brain," Nat. Rev. Neurosci., Vol. 2, No. 10, pp. 685-694, 2001. https://doi.org/10.1038/35094500
- W. M. Kelley, C. N. Macrae, C. L. Wyland, S. Caglar, S. Inati, and T. F. Heatherton, "Finding the self? An event-related fMRI study," J. Cogn. Neurosci., Vol. 14, No. 5, pp. 785-794, 2002. https://doi.org/10.1162/08989290260138672
- H. L. Gallagher, A. I. Jack, A. Roepstorff, and C. D. Frith, "Imaging the intentional stance in a competitive game," Neuroimage, Vol. 16, No. 3, Pt 1, pp. 814-821, 2002. https://doi.org/10.1006/nimg.2002.1117
- J. D. Greene, R. B. Sommerville, L. E. Nystrom, J. M. Darley, and J. D. Cohen, "An fMRI investigation of emotional engagement in moral judgment," Science, Vol. 293, No. 5537, pp. 2105-2108, 2001. https://doi.org/10.1126/science.1062872
- J. P. Mitchell, C. N. Macrae, and M. R. Banaji, "Dissociable medial prefrontal contributions to judgments of similar and dissimilar others," Neuron, Vol. 50, No. 4, pp. 655-663, 2006. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuron.2006.03.040
- N. Liberman and Y. Trope, "The psychology of transcending the here and now," Science, Vol. 322, No. 5905, pp. 1201-1205, 2008. https://doi.org/10.1126/science.1161958
- Y. Bar-Anan, N. Liberman, Y. Trope, and D. Algom, "Automatic processing of psychological distance: evidence from a Stroop task," J. Exp. Psychol. Gen., Vol. 136, No. 4, pp. 610-622, 2007. https://doi.org/10.1037/0096-3445.136.4.610
- A. L. Alter and D. M. Oppenheimer, "Effects of fluency on psychological distance and mental construal (or why New York is a large city, but New York is a civilized jungle)," Psychol. Sci., Vol. 19, No. 2, pp. 161-167, 2008. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-9280.2008.02062.x
- H. Song and N. Schwarz, "If it's difficult to pronounce, it must be risky," Psychol. Sci., Vol. 20, No. 2, pp. 135-138, 2009. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-9280.2009.02267.x
- A. Zajonc, "Attitudinal effexts of mere exposure," J. Pers. Soc. Psychol., Vol. 9, pp. 1-27, 1968.
- M. Wittmann and M. P. Paulus, "Decision making, impulsivity and time perception," Trends. Cogn. Sci., Vol. 12, No. 1, pp. 7-12, 2008. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.tics.2007.10.004