참고문헌
- Aikenhead, G. S. (2006). Science Education for Everyday Life: Evidence-based practice. New York: Teachers College Press
- Aliseda, A. (2004). Logics in scientific discovery. Foundations of Science, 9, 339-363 https://doi.org/10.1023/B:FODA.0000042847.62285.81
- Anderson, J. R. (2000). Cognitive psychology and its Implication. 6th ed. New York: Worth Publishers
- Andersson, B. (1986). The experiential gestalt of causation: a common core to pupils' preconceptions in science. European Journal of Science Education, 8(2), 155-171 https://doi.org/10.1080/0140528860080205
- Brooks, L. R. (1987). Non-analytic cognition. In U. Neisser (Ed.), Concepts and conceptual development: Ecological and intellectual bases of categories. Cambridge, MA: Cambridge University Press
- Chen, Z. (1996). Children's analogical problem solving: The effects of superficial, structural, and procedural similarity. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 62, 410-431 https://doi.org/10.1006/jecp.1996.0037
- Chen, Z., & Daehler, M. W. (1992). Intention and outcome: Key components of causal structure facilitating mapping in children's analogical transfer. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 53, 237-257 https://doi.org/10.1016/0022-0965(92)90038-8
- Crisafi, M. A., & Brown, A. L. (1986). Analogical transfer in very young children: Combining two separately learned solutions to reach a goal. Child Development, 57, 953-968 https://doi.org/10.2307/1130371
- Curd, M. V. (1980). The logic of discovery: an analysis of three approaches. In B. A. Brody & R. E. Grandy(1989) (Eds.), Readings in the philosophy of science (pp. 417-430). Englewood Cliffs, NJ: Prentice Hall
- Driver, R., Guesne, E., & Tiberghien, A. (1985). Children's ideas in science. Milton Keynes, Philadelphia: Open University Press
- Duit, R. (1991). Students' conceptual frameworks: Consequences for learning science. In S. M. Glynn, R. H. Yeany, & B. K. Britton (Eds.), The psychology of learning science (pp. 65-85). NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum
- Else, M. J., Clement, J., & Ramirez, M. A. (2003). Should different types of analogies be treated differently in instruction?: Observation from a middleschool life science curriculum. Paper presented at the Annual Meeting of the National Association for Research in Science Teaching, Philadelphia, PA
- Fischer, H. R. (2001). Abductive reasoning as a way of worldmaking. Foundation of Science, 6, 361-383
- Fleer, M. (1996). Fusing the boundaries between home and child care to support children's scientific learning. Research in Science Education, 16(2), 143-154
- Fleer, M., & Robbins, J. (2003). "Hit and run research" with "hit and miss" results in early childhood science education. Research in Science Education, 33(4), 405-431 https://doi.org/10.1023/B:RISE.0000005249.45909.93
- French, L. (2004). Science as the center of coherent, integrated early childhood curriculum. Early Childhood Research Quarterly, 19(1), 138-149 https://doi.org/10.1016/j.ecresq.2004.01.004
- Gilbert, J. K., Osborne, R. J., & Fensham, P. J. (1982). Children's science and its consequences for teaching. Science Education 66(4), 623-633 https://doi.org/10.1002/sce.3730660412
- Gilbert, J. K., & Watts, D. M. (1983). Concepts, misconception and alternative conceptions: changing perspectives in science education, Studies in Science Education, 10, 61-98 https://doi.org/10.1080/03057268308559905
- Hanson, N. R. (1958). Patterns of discovery. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press
- Hanson, N. R. (1961). Is there a logic of scientific discovery? In B. A. Brody & R. E. Grandy (1989) (Eds.) Readings in the philosophy of science (pp. 398-409). Englewood Cliffs, NJ: Prentice Hall
- Hinton, G. E., McClelland, J. L., & Rumelhart, D. E. (1986). Distributed representations. In D. E. Rumelhart & J. L. McClelland (Eds.), Parallel distributed processing: Exploration in the microstructure of cognition, Vol. 1 (pp. 77-109). Cambridge, MA: MIT Press
- Howard, E. W. (1987). Concepts and schemata. London: Cassell Educational
- Inagaki, K. (1992). Piagetian and post-Piagetian conceptions of development and their implications for science education in early childhood. Early Childhood Research Quarterly, 7, 115-133 https://doi.org/10.1016/0885-2006(92)90022-Q
- Jeong, J. -S., Won, H. -J., & Kwon, Y. -J. (2005). Application of the Triple Abduction Model for improving the skills of scientific hypothesis generation. Journal of the Korean Association for Research in Science Education, 25(5), 595-602
- Joung, Y. J. (2006). What do children do in everyday life to construct their scientific knowledge?: A case stud of a 5-year-old's experience. Proceedings of the International Science Education Conference 2006 (pp. 960-969), 22 nd - 24 th November 2006, Singapore
- Joung, Y. J., & Song, J. (2006a). The features of the hypotheses generated by pre-service elementary teachers using the form of Peirce's abduction. Journal of Korean Elementary Science Education, 25(2), 126-140
- Joung, Y. J., & Song, J. (2006b). Exploring the implications of Pierce's abduction in science education by theoretical investigation. Journal of the Korean Association for Research in Science Education, 26(6), 703-722
- Jung, Y. -J., & Song, J. (2004). An analysis of the features of 'Typically-Perceived-Situation (TPS)' for in-depth understanding of students' ideas: The case of four elementary school students' TPSs related to the action of force. Journal of the Korean Association for Research in Science Education, 24(4), 785-803
- Kang, T. (2006). The characteristics of Typically Perceived Situations (TPSs) and Critical Examples: Focusing on the secondary students' ideas of force and mechanical conversion. Unpublished MEd thesis. Seoul National University
- Kim, J. Y., & Kang, S. H. (2006). The practical use of process skill and the perception about hypothesis by secondary school science teachers. Journal of the Korean Association for Research in Science Education, 26(2), 258-267
- Kim, K., Shin, E., Byun, S., & Noh, T. (2006). Analysis of students' mapping errors induced in learning chemistry concept with analogy. Journal of the Korean Association for Research in Science Education, 26(4), 592-600
- Kaufman, D.R., Vosniadou, S., diSessa, A. & Thagard, P. (2000). Scientific Explanation, Systematicity, and Conceptual Change. In L. Gleitman & A.K. Joshi (eds.), Proceedings of the Twenty-First Annual Meeting of the Cognitive Science Society (pp. 5-9). Mawah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates
- Kwon, Y. -J, Jeong, J. -S., Kang, M. -J., & Kim, Y. -S. (2003a). A grounded theory on the process of generating hypothesis: Knowledge about scientific episodes. Journal of the Korean Association for Research in Science Education, 23(5), 458-469
- Kwon, Y. -J, Jeong, J. -S., Park, Y, -B., and Kang, M. -J. (2003b). A philosophical study on the generating process of declarative scientific knowledge: Focused on inductive, abductive, and deductive process. Journal of the Korean Association for Research in Science Education, 23(3), 215-228
- Lawson, A. E. (1995). Science Teaching and the Development of Thinking. Belmont, CA: Wadsworth Publishing Company
- Lawson, A. E. (2002). What does Galileo's discovery of Jupiter's moons tell us about the process of scientific discovery?. Science & Education, 11(1), 1-24 https://doi.org/10.1023/A:1013048828150
- Lemke, J. L. (2001). Articulating communities: Sociocultural perspectives on science education. Journal of Research in Science Teaching, 38(3), 296-316 https://doi.org/10.1002/1098-2736(200103)38:3<296::AID-TEA1007>3.0.CO;2-R
- Magnani, L. (2004). Model and manipulative abduction in science. Foundation of Science, 9(3), 219-247
- Medin, D. L. (1989). Concepts and conceptual structure. American Psychologist, 44(12), 1469-1481 https://doi.org/10.1037/0003-066X.44.12.1469
- Medin, D. L, & Schaffer, M. M. (1978). Context theory of classification learning. Psychological Review, 86, 207-238
- Merriam, S. B. (1988). Case study research in education: A qualitative approach. Jossey-Bass Publishers
- Myrstad, J. A. (2004). The use of converse abduction in Kepler. Foundation of Science, 9(3), 321-338
- Murphey, M. G. (1969). The development of Peirce's philosophy. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press
- Newell, A., & Simon, H. (1972). Human problem-solving. Prentice-Hall, Englewood Cliffs: NJ
- Oh, P. S. (2006). Rule-inferring strategies for abductive reasoning in the process of solving an earth-environmental problem. Journal of the Korean Association for Research in Science Education, 26(4), 546-558
- Oh, P. S., & Kim, C. J. (2005). A theoretical study on abduction as an inquiry method in earth science. Journal of the Korean Association for Research in Science Education, 25(5), 610-623
- Osborne, R. J., Bell, B. F., & Gilbert, J. K. (1983). Science teaching and children's views of the world. European Journal of Science Education, 5(1), 1-14 https://doi.org/10.1080/0140528830050101
- Osborne, R. J., & Freyberg, P. (1985). Learning in science: The implications of children's science. London: Heinemann
- Paavola, S. (2004). Abduction as a logic and methodology of discovery: The importance of strategies. Foundation of Science, 9(3), 267-283
- Park, E. M., & Kang, S. H. (2006). Effects of offering similar experiences for hypothesis-generation based on abduction. Journal of the Korean Association for Research in Science Education, 26(3), 356-366
- Park, J. (2000). Analysis of students' processes of generating scientific explanatory hypothesis: Focused on the definition and the characteristics of scientific hypothesis. Journal of the Korean Association for Research in Science Education, 20(4), 667-679
- Peirce, C. S. Collected Papers of Charles Sanders Peirce [abbr. CP], 8 vols. C. Hartshorne and P. Weiss(1931-1958) (Eds.) vols. 1-6; A. W. Burks (1931-1958) (Ed.) vols. 7-8, Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press
- Robbins, J. (2005). 'Brown paper packages'? A sociocultural perspective on young children's ideas in science. Research in Science Education, 35(2-3), 151-172 https://doi.org/10.1007/s11165-005-0092-x
- Rogoff, B. (1998). Cognition as a collaborative process. In W. Damon (Chief Ed.), D. Kuhn, & R. S. Siegler (Volume Eds.), Cognition, perceptions and language. 5 th Ed., Handbook of child psychology (pp. 679-744). New York: Wiley
- Rosch, E. (1978). Principles of categorization. In E. Rosch & B. Lloyd (Eds.), Cognition and categorization, Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum
- Rosch, E., & Marvis, C. B. (1975). Family resemblance: Studies in the internal structure of categories, Cognitive Psychology, 7, 573-605 https://doi.org/10.1016/0010-0285(75)90024-9
- Sherin, B. (2006). Common sense clarified: the role of intuitive knowledge in physics problem solving. Journal of Research in Science Teaching. 43(6). 535-555 https://doi.org/10.1002/tea.20136
- Shin, D. -H., & Kwon, Y. -J. (2006). Brain activities by the generating-process-types of scientific emotion in the pre-service teachers' hypothesis generation about biological phenomena: An fMRI study. Journal of Korean Association for Research in Science Education, 26(4), 568-580
- Thagard, P. (1988). Computational philosophy of science. Cambridge, MA: The MIT Press
- Thagard, P. (1992). Conceptual revolutions. NJ: Princeton University Press
- Vosniadou, S., & Brewer, W. (1987). Theories of Knowledge restructing in development. Review of Educational Research, 57(1), 51-67 https://doi.org/10.3102/00346543057001051
- Vygotsky, L. S. (1978). Mind in Society: The development of higher psychological processes (M. Cole, V. John-Steiner, S. Scribner, & E. Souberman, Eds.). Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press
- Yang, I. H., Jeong, J. S., Kwon, Y. J., Jeong, J. W., Hur, M., & Oh, C. H. (2006). An intensive interview study on the process of scientists` science knowledge generation. Journal of the Korean Association for Research in Science Education, 26(1), 88-98