The Relations of Preschoolers' Cortisol Pattern at Home to Tendency of Internalizing Behavior

유아의 가정에서의 코티솔 패턴과 내면화 행동성향 간의 관계

  • Lee, Young (Department of Child and Family Studies, Yonsei University) ;
  • Shin, Yee-Jin (Department of Psychiatry, College of Medicine) ;
  • Chung, Jee-Nha (Human Psychology Research Institute, Yonsei University) ;
  • Min, Sung-Hye (Department of Child Welfare, Namseoul University) ;
  • Min, Hyun-Suk (Department of Child and Family Studies, Yonsei University) ;
  • Lee, Kyung-Sook (Department of Rehabilitation, Hanshin University)
  • 이영 (연세대학교 아동.가족학과) ;
  • 신의진 (연세대학교 의과대학 정신의학교실) ;
  • 정지나 (연세대학교 생활과학연구소) ;
  • 민성혜 (남서울대학교 아동복지학과) ;
  • 민현숙 (연세대학교 아동.가족학과) ;
  • 이경숙 (한신대학교 재활학과)
  • Published : 2007.11.30

Abstract

The purposes of this study were to look at the patterns of Korean preschoolers' cortisol levels at home and to examine the relations of cortisol pattern to the preschooler's internalizing behavioral tendency with focused on behavioral inhibition and internalizing problematic behavior. The subjects of the study were forty 4-year old preschoolers(27 boys and 13 girls). Saliva was collected at home during the weekends, and cortisol was analyzed by Radioimmunoassay procedure. "Strange Peer Situation Experiment" developed by Rubin, Coplan, Fox and Calkins(1995) and revised by Rubin, Burgess and Hastings(2002) was used to measure the preschooler's behavior inhibition. Problematic behavior was measured through a "Korean Children's Behavior Checklist" (Oh, K. J., Lee, H. R., Hong, K. E., & Ha, E. H., 1997). The results were as follows: First, on average for a 4-year old, the level of cortisol was 0.22 in the morning, and 0.14 in the afternoon. There was a pattern that cortisol levels had declined in the afternoon. There was no difference in cortisol levels between genders. Secondly, there was a tendency that children of higher inhibition level at the lab showed the higher cortisol levels in the morning. However, this difference was not statistically significant. Thirdly, the higher the cortisol levels at home, the more showed internalizing behavior. Externalizing behavior was not correlated with the cortisol levels. It was concluded that cortisol may be related to children's internalizing behavior tendency.

Keywords

References

  1. 민성혜(2005). 유아 내적 표상과 행동억제. 연세대학교 박사학위논문
  2. 서수경(1993). 유아의 행동과 기질 및 성차에 관한 연구. 이화여자대학교 석사학위논문
  3. 오경자, 이혜련, 홍강의, 하은혜(1997). 아동 . 청소년 행동평가 척도(K-CBCL). 서울: 중앙적성연구소
  4. Achenbach, T. M., & Edelbrock, C.(1986). Manual for the Teacher's Report Form and Teacher Version of the Child Behavior Profile. Bar lington, VT: University of Vermont
  5. Belsky, J., Fish, M., & Isabella, R.(1991). Continuity and discontinuity in infant negative and positive emotionality: Family antecedents and attachment consequences. Developmental Psychology, 27, 421-431 https://doi.org/10.1037/0012-1649.27.3.421
  6. Cacioppo, J. T.(1994). Social neuroscience: Autonomic, neuroendocrine, and immune responses to stress. Psychophysiology, 31, 113-128 https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1469-8986.1994.tb01032.x
  7. Calkins, S. D., & Fox, N. A.(1992). The relations among infant temperament, security of attachment, and behavioral inhibition at twenty-four months. Child Development, 63, 1456-1472 https://doi.org/10.2307/1131568
  8. de Hann, M., Gunnar, M. R., Tout, K., Hart, J., & Stansbury, K.(1998). Familiar and novel contexts yield different associations between cortisol and behavior among 2-year-olds. Developmental Psychobiology, 33, 93-101 https://doi.org/10.1002/(SICI)1098-2302(199807)33:1<93::AID-DEV8>3.0.CO;2-N
  9. de Kloet, E. R.(1991). Brain corticosteroid receptor balance and homeostatic control. Frontiers in Neuroendocrinology, 12, 93-101
  10. Dettling, A. C., Gunnar, M. R., & Donzella, B.(1999). Cortisol levels of young children in full-day childcare centers: Relations with age and temperament. Psychoneuroendocrinology, 24, 519-536 https://doi.org/10.1016/S0306-4530(99)00009-8
  11. Dettling, A. C., Parker, S. W., Lane, S. K., Sebanc, A. M., & Gunnar, M. R.(2000). Quality of care and temperament determine whether cortisol levels rise over the day for children in full-day child care. Psychoneuroendocrinology, 25, 819-836 https://doi.org/10.1016/S0306-4530(00)00028-7
  12. Durbrow, E., Gunnar, M. R., Bozoky, I., Adam, E., Jimerson, S., & Chen, C.(1998). The stress of going to school: Variations in adrenocortical reactivity in a rural Caribbean village. Manuscript submitted for publication
  13. Fox, N. A. Henderson, H. A, Marshall, P. J, Nichols, K. E., & Ghera, M. M.(2005). Behavioral inhibition: Linking biology and behavior within a developmental framework. Annual Review of Psychology, 56, 235-262 https://doi.org/10.1146/annurev.psych.55.090902.141532
  14. Fox, N. A., Rubin, K. H., Calkins, S. D., Marshall, T. R., Coplan, R. J., Porges, S. W., Long, J. M., & Stewart, S.(1995). Frontal activation asymmetry and social competence at four years of age. Child Development, 66, 1770-1784 https://doi.org/10.2307/1131909
  15. Friend, M., & Davis, T. L.(1993). Appearance-reality distinction: Children's understanding of the physical and affective domains. Developmental Psychology, 29, 907-914 https://doi.org/10.1037/0012-1649.29.5.907
  16. Granger, D. A., Weisz, J. R., McCracken, J. T., Ikeda, S. C., & Douglas, P .(1996). Reciprocal influences among adrenocortical activation, psycholosial processes, and the behavioral adjustment of clinic-referred children. Child Development, 67, 3250-3262 https://doi.org/10.2307/1131777
  17. Gunnar, M. R., Tout, K., deHaan, M., Pierce,S., & Stansbury, K.(1997). Temperament, social competence, and adrenocortical activity in preschoolers. Developmental Psychobiology, 31, 65-85 https://doi.org/10.1002/(SICI)1098-2302(199707)31:1<65::AID-DEV6>3.0.CO;2-S
  18. Heim, C., Owen, M:J., Plotsky, P. M., & Nemeroff, C. B.(1997). The role of early adverse life events in the etiology of depression and posttraumatic stress disorder: Focus on corticotropin-releasing factor. Annals of the New York Academy of Science, 821, 194-207 https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1749-6632.1997.tb48279.x
  19. Kagan, J., Reznick, J. S., Clarke, C., Snidman, N., & Garcia Coll.(1984). Behavioral inhibition to the unfamiliar. Child Development, 55, 2212-2225 https://doi.org/10.2307/1129793
  20. Kagan, J., Reznick, J. S., & Snidman, N.(1987). The physiology and psychology of behavioral inhibition in children. Child Development, 58, 1459-1473 https://doi.org/10.2307/1130685
  21. Kagan, J., Reznick, J. S., Snidman, H., Gibbons, J., & Johnson, M.(1988). Childhood derivatives of inhibition and lack of inhibition to the unfamiliar. Child Development, 59, 1580-1589 https://doi.org/10.2307/1130672
  22. Nachmias, M., Gunnar, M., Mangelsdorf., Parritz, R. H., & Buss, K.(1996). Behavioral Inhibition and Stress Reactivity: The Moderation Role of Attachment Security, Child Development, 67, 508-522 https://doi.org/10.2307/1131829
  23. Park, S, Y., Belsky, J., Putnam,S., & Crnic, K.(1997). Infant emotionality, parenting, and 3-year inhibition: Exploring stability and lawful discontinuity in a male sample. Developmental Psychology, 33, 218-227 https://doi.org/10.1037/0012-1649.33.2.218
  24. Rubin, K. H., Burgess, K. B., & Hastings, P. D.(2002). Stability and social-behavioral consequences of toddlers' inhibited temperament and parenting behaviors. Child Development, 73, 483-495 https://doi.org/10.1111/1467-8624.00419
  25. Rubin, K. H., Coplan, R. J., Fox, N. A., & Calkins, S. D.(1995). Emotionality, emotion regulation, and preschoolers' social adaptation. Developmental Psychopathology, 7,49-62 https://doi.org/10.1017/S0954579400006337
  26. Rubin, K. H., & Mills, R. S. L.(1990). Maternal beliefs about adaptive and maladaptive social behaviors in normal, aggressive, and withdrawn preschoolers. Journal of Abnormal Child psychology, 18, 419-435 https://doi.org/10.1007/BF00917644
  27. Schmidt, L. A., Fox, N. A., Rubin, K. H., Sternberg, E. M., Gold, P. W., Smith, C. C., & Schulkin, J. (1997). Behavioral and neuroendocrine responses in shy children. Developmental Psychobiology, 30, 127-140 https://doi.org/10.1002/(SICI)1098-2302(199703)30:2<127::AID-DEV4>3.0.CO;2-S
  28. Spangler, G., & Scheubeck, R.(1993). Behavioral organization in newborns and its relation to adrenocortical and cardiac activity. Child Development, 64, 622-633 https://doi.org/10.2307/1131273
  29. Stansbury, K., & Gunnar, M.(1995). Adrenocortical activity and emotion regulation. In N. Fox(Ed.), The development of emotion regulation: Biological and behavioral considerations(pp. 108-134). Monographs of the Society for Research in Child Development, 59(2-3, Serial No. 240)
  30. Stevenson-Hinde, J., & Shouldice, A.(1995). 4.5 to 7 years: Fearful behavior, fears and worries. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 36, 1027-1038 https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1469-7610.1995.tb01348.x
  31. Tout, K., de Haan, M., Campbell, E. K., & Gunnar, M. R.(1998). Social Behavior Correlates of Cortisol Activity in Child Care: Gender Differences and Time-of-Day Effects, Child Development, 69, 1247-1262 https://doi.org/10.2307/1132263
  32. Watamura, S. E.(2007). 스트레스가 뇌에 미치는 영향. 삼성복지재단 제 15회 국제학술대회 건강한 환경, 유능한 어린이 자료집(pp.35-84)
  33. Watamura, S. E., Donzella, B., Alwin, J., & Gunnar, 'M. R.(2003). Morning-to-Afternoon Increses in Cortisol Concentrations for Infants and Toddlers at Child Care; Age Differences and Behavioral Correlates. Child Development, 74, 1006-1020 https://doi.org/10.1111/1467-8624.00583
  34. Watamura, S. E., Donzella, B., Kertes, D. A., & Gunnar, M. R.(2004). Developmental changes in baseline cortisol activity on early childhood: Relations with napping and effortful control. Developmental psychobiology, 45, 125-133 https://doi.org/10.1002/dev.20026
  35. Watamura, S. E., Sebanc, A. M., & Gunnar, M. R.(2002). Rising Cortisol at Childcare; Relations With Nap, Rest, and Temperament, Developmental Psychobiology, 40, 33-42 https://doi.org/10.1002/dev.10011